AUTHOR: Biomed Mom TITLE: EFAs and behavior DATE: 9/08/2007 07:08:00 AM ----- BODY:
What are EFAs? · Nutrition: Essential Fatty Acids (EFAs): What are EFAs? Things to Avoid · Nutrition: Essential Fatty Acids (EFAs): Things to Avoid Omega-3 Deficiency Symptoms List of Symptoms "These signs include · dry hair, · dry skin (often noticed as a 'goosebump' rash on the upper arms and/or upper thighs), · excessive thirst, · frequent urination, · problems with attention and so on." ILT: Essential Fatty Acids (EFAs) Keratosis Pilaris Keratosis Pilaris: Definition "Keratosis pilaris is a common skin condition that looks like small goose bumps, which are actually dead skin cells that build up around the hair follicle." HealthAtoZ.com: Keratosis Pilaris Keratosis Pilaris: Description · "Keratosis pilaris is a disorder that occurs around the hair follicles of the upper arms, thighs, and sometimes the buttocks. · It presents as small, benign bumps or papules that are actually waxy build-ups of keratin. Normally skin sloughs off. However, around the hair follicle where the papules form, the keratinized skin cells slough off at a slower rate, clogging the follicles. · This is generally thought to be genetic disorder, although the symptoms of keratosis pilaris are often seen with ichthyosis and allergic dermatitis. It can also be observed in people of all ages who have either inherited it or have a vitamin A deficiency or have dry skin. · Keratosis pilaris is a self-limiting disorder that disappears as the person ages. · It can become more severe when conditions are dry such as during the winter months or in dry climates." HealthAtoZ.com: Keratosis Pilaris Keratosis Pilaris: Causes and Symptoms "The specific causes of this disorder are unknown. Since this disorder runs in families, it is thought to be hereditary. Keratosis pilaris is not a serious disorder and is not contagious. The symptoms of keratosis pilaris are based on the development of small white papules the size of a grain of sand on the upper arms, thighs, and occasionally the buttocks and face. The papules occur around a hair follicle and are firm and white. They feel a little like coarse sandpaper, but they are not painful and there usually is no itching associated with them. They are easily removed and the material inside the papule usually contains a small, coiled hair." HealthAtoZ.com: Keratosis Pilaris Keratosis Pilaris: Treatment · "To treat keratosis pilaris patients can try several strategies to lessen the bumps. · First, the patient can supplement the natural removal of dry skin and papules by using a loofah or another type of scrub showering or bathing. · A variety of different over-the-counter (OTC) lotions, ointments, and creams can also be applied after showering while the skin is still moist and then several times a day to keep the area moist. · Medicated lotions with urea, 15% alphahydroxy acids, or Retin A can also be prescribed by the dermatologist and applied one to two times daily. · Systemic (oral) medications are not prescribed for keratosis pilaris. · However if papules are opened and become infected, antibiotics may be necessary to treat the infection." HealthAtoZ.com: Keratosis Pilaris "I did a little research on vitamin a deficiency and it seems to be, if not a cause, then a trigger to a lot of skin problems. However, taking vitamin A does not necessarily eliminate the deficiency. I know this because I was taking a vitamin A/B combo and nothing was happening. Then after doing some reading about eczema…omega 3 fatty acids (found in cod liver oil supplements [caution: cod liver oil in the quantity necessary to get three grams per day of omega-3's would contain dangerous levels of vitamin A]) help the body absorb vitamin A. This has worked for me and has offset a lot of the uncomfortable dry skin associated with not only my eczema, but also on my arms and legs (KP)." KeratosisPilaris.org: Archives: Re: Vitamin A and KP · Essential Fatty Acids (EFAs): Things to Avoid: Too Much Fish, Farm-Raised Fish · Essential Fatty Acids (EFAs): Things to Avoid: Dangerous Levels of Vitamin A · Nutrition: Vitamins and Minerals: Liquid Multivitamins: Absorption · Nutrition: Vitamins and Minerals: Vitamin A: Absorption Effects ADD, Dyslexia "If a child does not get enough EFAs, the myelin sheath protecting the axons of billions of neurons may not be adequate. Recent research is showing that boys with ADD have EFA deficiencies (Stevens, L. et al, 1995:000-000) and similar deficiencies are possibly responsible for the symptoms of dyslexia. (Stordy & Nicholl, 2000:105)." ILT: Essential Fatty Acids (EFAs) Depression "When researchers monitor eating habits across countries, they find that as fish consumption goes up, depression rates go down. In fact, there is a sixty-fold difference in depression rates across countries from the highest (Japan and Taiwan) to the lowest (North America, Europe and New Zealand) omega-3 fat consumption. Even postpartum depression decreases as women increase their consumption of fish. Many people also report a drop in mood when they switch too quickly to a low-fat diet. (Page 156)" Book: Somer, Elizabeth, M.A., R.D. Food & Mood. Henry Holt and Company, LLC, 1999. Serotonin and Dopamine "Over the last decade, neuroscientists have been examining the consequences of omega-3 deficiencies in the central nervous system. Alterations in serotonin and dopamine levels, as well as the functioning of these two important neurotransmitters is evident in an omega-3 deficiency. The changes observed in omega-3 deficiency in animals is strikingly similar to that found in autopsy studies of human depression.20" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Blood-Brain Barrier "In addition to changing serotonin and dopamine levels and functioning, omega-3 deficiencies are known to compromise the blood-brain barrier, which normally protects the brain from unwanted matter gaining access.21" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Blood Flow "Omega-3 deficiency can also decrease normal blood flow to the brain,22,23 an interesting finding given the studies which show that patients with depression have compromised blood flow to a number of brain regions.24,25" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Antidepressant Activity "Finally, omega-3 deficiency also causes a 35 percent reduction in brain phosphatidylserine (PS) levels.26 This is also of relevance when considering that PS has documented antidepressant activity in humans.27,28" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Mechanisms of EPA/DHA Regulation of Mood "DHA [an omega-3 fatty acid] is found in high levels in the cells of the central nervous system (neurons); here it acts as a form of scaffolding for structural support.29 When omega-3 intake is inadequate, the nerve cell becomes stiff as cholesterol and omega-6 fatty acids are substituted for omega-3.30 When a nerve cell becomes rigid, proper neurotransmission from cell to cell and within cells will be compromised.31 While DHA provides structure and helps to ensure normal neurotransmission, EPA [an omega-3 fatty acid] may be more important in the signaling within nerve cells.32 Normalizing communications within nerve cells has been suggested to be an important factor in alleviating depressive symptoms.33" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH "In addition, EPA can lower the levels of two important immune chemicals, · tumour necrosis factor alpha (TNFa) and · interleukin 1 beta (IL-1ß), · as well as prostaglandin E2.34 All three of these chemicals are elevated in depression.35-38 In fact, higher levels of TNFa and IL-1ß are associated with severity of depression.39" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH "Finally, EPA has been hypothesized to increase brain-derived neurotropic factor (BDNF), which is known to be lower in depressed patients.20 BDNF is neuroprotective, enhances neurotransmission, has antidepressant activity and supports normal brain structure. BDNF may prevent the death of nerve cells in depression." New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Correcting an Omega-3 Deficiency Note: More information about sources of omega-3s can be found on the page Nutrition: Essential Fatty Acid (EFA) Sources Flax "Flax, our richest source of omega 3, quickly replenishes a long-standing omega 3 deficiency. A dozen 8 oz. bottles of good quality flax oil consumed over the course of a few months will suffice [approximately one bottle every five days, 1.6 ounces per day]." The Edelson Center: Essential Fatty Acids: The Healing Fats "There have been some published case reports indicating that flaxseed oil may be helpful in cases of bipolar depression and the anxiety disorder agoraphobia.40 The first controlled clinical trial indicating that omega-3 fatty acids may be of benefit in depression was published in 1999. In this case, 9:6 g ['9:6 g' should be read a dose of 6 g per day for a period of nine days?] of EPA/DHA versus placebo led to longer periods of remission and improvement in depressive symptoms in those with bipolar depression.41" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH 2g Daily of Pure EPA "Some researchers theorize that such high doses of EPA/DHA may not be necessary and that low levels of pure EPA may be of benefit.32 In a study published in the American Journal of Psychiatry, researchers showed that just 2g of pure EPA could improve the symptoms of treatment-resistant depression. The researchers found that the EPA (versus placebo), when added to an ineffective antidepressant for one month, significantly improved depressive symptoms.42" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH 1g Daily of Pure EPA "A larger study published in Archives of General Psychiatry replicated these findings, however, this time various doses of EPA were examined. Those on ineffective antidepressants were given 1g, 2g or 4g of pure EPA or a placebo in addition to the medication. Interestingly, the 1g daily dose of EPA led to the most significant improvements over the three-month study; it appeared that less was more. There were significant improvements in depressive symptoms, sleep, anxiety, lassitude, libido and thoughts of suicide.43" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH "Harvard researchers have also shown that just 1g of pure EPA is beneficial in the treatment of borderline personality disorder. This personality disorder, which is particularly difficult to treat, is characterized by both depressive and aggressive symptoms. This was a two-month placebo-controlled study and the results showed that EPA has a mood-regulating effect, improving both depression and aggression versus placebo.46" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH 4.4g EPA and 2.2g DHA Mix "Researchers from Taiwan Medical University published a recent study in which they found that a 4.4g EPA and 2.2g DHA mix could alleviate depression versus placebo in those with treatment-resistant depression. This was a two-month study involving patients who were on antidepressants that were not working. As with the other omega-3 studies discussed, the fish oil was well tolerated and no adverse events were reported.44" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Antarctic Krill Oil (400mg EPA, 240mg DHA) "There is also evidence that omega-3 oils may be of benefit in treating depressive symptoms outside of major depressive disorder. Canadian researchers showed that Antarctic krill oil (400mg EPA, 240mg DHA) could improve depressive symptoms associated with premenstrual syndrome.45" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH DHA Alone not Recommended "To date, with one exception, all studies conducted on omega-3 fatty acids and mood have had a positive outcome. The singular negative study examined pure DHA in patients with depression. The results in the case showed that DHA alone was no better than placebo in alleviating depressive symptoms.47" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Omega-6 Deficiency "Long--term exclusion or excessive use of flax oil can result in omega 6 deficiency after about two years, because flax oil is omega 3 rich but omega 6 poor." The Edelson Center: Essential Fatty Acids: The Healing Fats

Labels: , , , , ,

----- -------- AUTHOR: Biomed Mom TITLE: Low Dopamine in Neglected/Abused kids? DATE: 8/22/2007 11:04:00 AM ----- BODY:
Gerra G, Leonardi C, Cortese E, Zaimovic A, Dell'agnello G, Manfredini M, Somaini L, Petracca F, Caretti V, Saracino MA, Raggi MA, Donnini C Homovanillic acid (HVA) plasma levels inversely correlate with attention deficit-hyperactivity and childhood neglect measures in addicted patients. J Neural Transm. 2007 Aug 10; Background. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) seems to be a risk condition for substance use disorders, possibly in relationship to common neurobiological changes, underlying both addictive and externalising behaviour susceptibility. Although this vulnerability has been primarily attributed to gene variants, previous studies suggest that also adverse childhood experiences may influence neurotransmission, affecting in particular brain dopamine (DA) system and possibly concurring to the development of behavioural disorders. Therefore, we decided to investigate ADHD symptoms and plasma concentrations of the DA metabolite homovanillic acid (HVA) in abstinent addicted patients, in comparison with healthy control subjects, evaluating whether ADHD scores were related with HVA levels, as expression of DA turnover, and whether HVA values, in turn, were associated with childhood emotional neglect. Methods. Eighty-two abstinent drug dependent patients, and 44 normal controls, matched for age and sex, completed the Wender Utah Rating Scale (WURS), measuring ADHD symptoms, and the Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse Questionnaire (CECA-Q). Blood samples were collected to determine HVA plasma levels. Results. Addicted individuals showed significantly higher ADHD scores and lower HVA levels respect to control subjects. ADHD scores at WURS in addicted patients negatively correlated with plasma HVA values. In turn, plasma HVA levels were inversely associated with childhood neglect measures, reaching statistical significance with "mother-antipathy" and "mother neglect" scores. Conclusions. These findings suggest the possibility that childhood experience of neglect and poor mother-child attachment may have an effect on central dopamine function as an adult, in turn contributing to both ADHD and substance abuse neurobiological vulnerability.

Labels: , , ,

----- -------- AUTHOR: Biomed Mom TITLE: Cofactors for neurotransmitters DATE: 7/18/2007 11:03:00 AM ----- BODY:
Tourette Syndrome, B6 dependency, allergy BonnieGr bonniegr at aol.com Thu Feb 8 13:19:55 EST 1996 I have been putting the puzzle pieces together on the subject of Tourette Syndrome, using medline documents, college textbooks, etc. Please read the following long rationale document that I have prepared, and comment by e-mail (BonnieGr at aol.com). It is my hope that more research will be done to validate my theory. Enjoy! The following addresses TS/OCD/ADD. The diagnosis would lie in the degree of vitamin B6 dependency/deficiency, and how long the person has been in this state. Carl Hansen, Jr. M.D. of Minneapolis describes celiac disease in several of his TS patients. This could be a pathway to vitamin B6 deficiency via malabsorption. Streptococcal infections have also been associated with TS. This could be a combination of the hyaluronidase's (an enzyme produced by the hemolytic strep that depolymerizes the ground substance of tissue) or streptokinase's actions on the blood brain barrier, the drain of vitamin B6 from the bacteria's own useage, the body's requirement of B6 for immunity, and the antibiotic's B6 antagonistic properties. A pre-exising B6 dependency/deficiency could be uncovered. TOURETTE SYNDROME, ALLERGY AND THE B6 DEPENDENCY STATE I have my Bachelor's degree in Biology, specializing in Medical Technology, and in graduate school, I took graduate courses in biochemistry. I work as a medical technologist performing and verifying clinical laboratory tests in Chemistry, Hematology and Blood Bank at Mt.Carmel East Medical Center in Columbus, Ohio. I do not have TS but my son, Jason (13 yrs old) has TS with OCD. ADD has not been formally diagnosed, although he has problems with organization, distractibility, and the ability to switch gears. My son has had allergies since he was a baby. He is sensitive to red dye #40 with tired splitting headaches which make him scream until he is exhausted and sleeps. This, of course, hasn't happened in several years since he has avoided the dye. He also is allergic to sulfa, molds, dust, grass, trees, and most airborn allergens. He has been on the vitamins below for 1.5 months and the teachers have said that he is a different kid. Medications that he had been on made him progressively worse, and so we made a personal decision to discontinue meds altogether. He now is motivated in school, concentrates and finishes his work, and is less disruptive with his tics in class. At home he still has his tics and compulsions, but they are shorter lived and occur less often. He has had a set back this week due to a new semester with a new schedule, plus a very moldy, rainy few days. We gave him a little extra calcium-magnesium and one extra vitamin B3. He said that this gave him relief from his symptoms (he has never said this before with anything else). I solidified my theory on the premise that Jason is probably mildly vitamin B6 dependent. He was either born requiring high amounts of B6, and/or B6 antagonists attacked early in his first year of life. B6 antagonists are hydrazines (plant growth regulators, tartrazine, etc), DOPA found in certain beans, penicillinamine, antioxidants in petroleum, many drugs including penicillin, erythromycin, phenobarbital, tetracycline, corticosteroids, sulfamethoxazole, etc. Amino acids began building up in his system, from decreased transamination, etc. Serotonin became decreased from tryptophan not being able to be utilized. Allergies developed (which is in association with low B6), I believe allergy produces swings in histamine levels which causes a constant fluctuation in neurotransmitters capable of producing mood swings and rages. The conservation of vitamin B6 (when not abundantly available) causes it to be used by the prevailing neurotransmitter system at any given time, leaving other neurotransmitter systems less than optimally functional. Histamine receptors have been found to trigger dopamine receptors directly. Histamine is also a neurotransmitter affected by deficient vitamin B6. Its receptor sites are probably increased to compensate. Kinins released into the body's tissues in response to immune complexes can damage the blood brain barrier, thus altering the sensitivity of brain cells to acetylcholine, serotonin, dopamine, histamine, epinephrine and norepinephrine. I found that L-dopa doesn't readily form dopamine in B6 deficiency, so probably dopamine is reduced causing an increase in dopamine receptor sites along with an increase the norepinephrine and epinephrine (which are formed from dopamine) receptors sites. These increased receptor sites make the nerves more excitable and false transmitters or true neurotransmitters can set them off with explosive qualities. These false transmitters can be phenolic substances, such as food additives, drugs, etc. The enzyme, phenol sulfotransferase (PST), detoxifies and eliminates phenolics (drugs, food additives, serotonin, dopamine (to name a few). In the brain, sulfation is used while glucuronidation prevails elsewhere. Cysteine requires B6 to enzymatically release sulfur for sulfation of these phenols by PST. Considering this, the neurotransmitters would would be conserved to a certain extent (their sulfation and elimination would be slowed down). ADD may happen when these false transmitters create background "noise", and if there is a real message to get through via other neurons, it is masked. When a true message is fired, it may have too strong of a signal, creating a strong impulsion, which can lead to the development of a tic or compulsion if the impulsion is acted upon and repeated creating a sort of conditioned reflex network of nerves. Mental, motor, and vocal tics can develop this way. According to my_ Biochemistry_ by Lehninger textbook from my graduate student in Biology days, tryptophan is broken down in Vitamin B3 deficiency to make nicotinic acid. Tryptophan is found in meat and is plentiful, if you are a meat eater. Tryptophan is the precursor for serotonin. I also looked up Vitamin B3 and how it could be connected to the issues of allergy and serotonin defiency in the brain. I found that Vitamin B3 is used to make NAD, NADP, which are coenzymes used in making histamine and serotonin (to name a few), and are essential in oxidative-reductive cellular metabolism. The B3 is needed due to tryptophan's inability to be broken down to nicotinic acid without adequate B6. So, if Vitamins B3 and B6 are being used for histamine production, then serotonin production suffers. Tryptophan then must be used in a higher frequency to make nicotinic acid. In Vitamin B6 deficiency, this cannot happen, because the enzyme kynurinase, that catalyzes the cleavage of 3 hydroxykynurine (an intermediate in tryptophan catabolism), contains pyridoxal phosphate (an active coenzyme form of Vitamin B6). In Vitamin B6 deficiency, large amounts of L-kynurenine are excreted in the urine, because of its high plasma levels. This is described in "Elevated plasma kynurenine in Tourette syndrome", _Molecular & Chemical Neuropathology_21(1): 55-60,1994 Jan. Kynurenine itself is metabolised to other substances, several of which are known to have effects on neurones. (per a research study done at University College London Medical School Harlow, England by Sheila L. Handley, BPharm, Ph.D. 1994) Large amounts of tryptophan which is broken down to ineffectively try to produce nicotinic acid reduces the amount of serotonin produced. Ineffective tryptophan utilization also uses alot of oxygen with tryptophan 2,3-dioxygenase. Low serotonin levels could cause obsessive compulsive behaviour, depression, and other mood related disorders. B6 is also required for the decarboxylase step of serotonin, histamine, and catecholamine pathways in the brain. In low B6, conservation takes place, so that B6 is used for fewer enzymes. When allergy strikes, the production of histamine causes a further imbalance of neurotransmitters, causing serotonin and/or catecholamine production to be further depleted. Sherry A Rogers, M.D., a specialist in environmental medicine, reports that all of the TS cases she has seen have a least one nutrient deficiency, and usually several. And she notes that all of these patients have hidden mold, dust, chemical and food sensitivities. ("Tourette Syndrome", _Health Counselor_, Vol.7, No.4) Acetylcholine is produced by acetyl CoA and choline. The choline is supplied through lecithin in Jason's supplements. In vitamin B6 deficiency, acetyl CoA would be made by fatty acid oxidation. So acetycholine could be functional with an adequate supply of fatty acids (evening primrose oil or flax oil might be useful). Acetylcholine could be in shorter supply in the parasympathetic system (relaxation) due to overuse in the sympathetic system where norepinephrine usually rules. The parasympathetic nervous system would need to have more acetylcholine in TS and associated disorders, it seems. Relaxation through the parasympathetic nervous system (which uses acetylcholine), where the heart rate is slowed, the blood pressure is lowered, the food is digested well, etc. is difficult in TS. Acetylcholine is probably overactive in the sympathetic autonomic nervous system, trying to stimulate the low supply of catecholamines, which would be decreased due to B6 deficiency/dependency. The receptors sites for catecholamines would be hyperexcitable and increased in number. The net usage of catecholamines could be normal to decreased due to increased stimulation by acetylcholine, depending on the availability of B6 in the body, and the conservation by low sulfation by PST. Conditions of emotional stress are known to produce more ticcing in TS. In short term stress, norepineprine, dopamine, and epineprine should be able to be produced by the conservation tactics of the body, but in long term stress, these would be exhausted, especially when another B6 dependent system is triggered. Likewise, the same would happen when histamine and serotonin are produced in short term and long term allergy. But as you might expect, the short term conditions would be explosive events with all of those increased receptor sites! Acetylcholine is also involved in the contraction of voluntary muscle cells and many other motor nerves, which are in heavy use in TS. Many people with TS are helped by exercise, where cardiac output and increased body temperature over a period of time inhibit the sympathetic nervous system. It may also help to clear toxic waste, such as kynurenine. Adequate water intake would be required to catabolize acetylcholine by cholinesterase. In my opinion acetylcholine is needed in B6 deficiency/dependency to run the nervous system. Fatty acids are essential to its success in this situation. Fatty acids require NADPH2, and NADH2 for their synthesis, and thus Vitamin B3. Water is also an utmost requirement in keeping acetylcholine from becoming a continuous firecracker. Jason has a water bottle close by most times and drinks tons of water. Water has always calmed him down. It may also dilute the kynurenine, excess amino acids and promote their excretion. If you look at the material written on the Canadian Mennonite families that have been studied with Tourette's disorder, you will see a high frequency of autoimmune and rare conditions. These findings are consistent with what one can expect with other Tourette's patients. For example, there is a high frequency of allergic conditions. My informal survey of TS and allergy results from the online TS support group are: With a total of 25 respondents with TS: 96% have allergies (24 out of 25) 56% have mold allergies 72% have obsessive complulsive traits (18 out of 25) 67% of those with obsessive compulsive traits have mold allergies 3 respondents thought they may have mold allergies, but weren't sure 52% have pollen allergies (ragweed, grass, tree, etc) 56% of those with obsessive compulsive traits have pollen allergy 48 % have animal allergies (cats, dogs, horse) 39% of those with obsessive compulsive traits have animal allergies 40% have dust allergy 39% of those with obsessive compulsive traits have dust allergy 20% have penicillin allergy 28 % of those with obsessive compulsive traits have penicillin allergy 20 % have miscellaneous allergies 11% of those with obsessive compulsive traits have miscellaneous allergies 16 % have food allergies 22 % of those with obsessive complulsive traits have food allergies 8% have sulfa allergy 11% of those with obsessive compulsive traits have sulfa allergy All of our frequent posters responded. The types of allergies are typically respiratory and airborne. Molds and pollens are the top allergens. 79% of the people with mold allergies also had pollen allergies, which are seasonal. Bonnie Grimaldi, BSMT (ASCP) 11283 Meadowcroft St. Pickerington, Ohio 43147 (614) 837-7545

Labels: , , , , ,

----- -------- AUTHOR: Biomed Mom TITLE: Neuroscience -- biomed for neurotransmitters DATE: 7/02/2007 06:17:00 PM ----- BODY:
looksmart FindArticles > Townsend Letter for Doctors and Patients > Jan, 2007 > Article > Print friendly Neurotransmitter Balancing, implemented properly: an indispensable clinical tool Pam Machemehl Helmly Introduction Identifying and treating neurotransmitter (NT) deficiencies and imbalances represent a vast new frontier in upgrading health care. It's only in the last five years that easy-to-use, accurate, and noninvasive tests for neurotransmitter deficiencies have become available. Yet, such tests are under-utilized, and many practitioners remain unfamiliar with NT treatment. Even though it's relatively new, a number of studies in this burgeoning field confirm its powerful effects. (1-2) [ILLUSTRATION OMITTED] A recent study of 78 Russian orphans (adopted into the US), conducted by Dr. Karyn Purvis at Texas Christian University, demonstrated treatment efficacy. The children had severe behavioral disorders, triggered in part by NT imbalances, which were identified through urine testing and addressed through supplementation. In the study, the children were divided into two groups. Immediately after initial testing, the treatment group received neurotransmitter support for two months. The control group was tested, but then waited two months before receiving the supplements. After two months, the treatment group showed significant improvements in both their neurotransmitter test results and in certain tested behaviors including anxiety/depression, thought process, attention problems, and aggressive behavior. "These improvements suggest that amino acid therapy has promise as an intervention for behaviorally disordered children," the researchers concluded. The study findings have been published in the Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine. Dr. Purvis' research (3) confirms what I've observed in my own work with over 4,200 clients in the last eight years: a significant part of a patient's emotional health is determined by his (or her) brain chemistry. Achieving psychological balance is just one of the countless health benefits attainable through addressing brain chemistry. NT imbalances cause (and contribute to) an astonishingly wide range of ailments and symptoms, including depression, anxiety, ADD/ADHD, fatigue, lack of drive, restlessness, anxiety, sleep cycle disorders, migraine headaches, palpitations, immune system suppression, hormonal imbalance, focus issues, memory loss, poor coordination and motor skills, weight gain, food cravings, eating disorders, weight loss, addictions, and chronic pain. Many of these symptoms undermine a patient's ability to comply with health care regimens, making it crucial to address underlying NT issues early on. Yet many clinicians lack the know-how to do so successfully. As one of the few clinicians with a full-time practice devoted exclusively to this specialty, I've been active in this field since its infancy. I founded my Texas-based online company, Neurogistics, to meet the needs of both clinicians and patients. For testing, we rely on labs that utilize the authentic German assays supported by normative data to ensure accurate results and optimum ranges. Moreover, all protocols we generate are based on an extensive logic I developed and individually fine-tune for each patient to insure that nothing is missed, and our scientific team reviews every protocol. Neurotransmitter Depletions in Brief Neurotransmitters are brain chemicals that communicate information throughout the brain and body, relaying signals between neurons. Functioning in dynamic balance are two kinds of neurotransmitters: the excitatory (such as norepinephrine), which stimulate, and the inhibitory (such as serotonin), which calm the brain to balance mood. When the excitatory neurotransmitters are overactive, the inhibitory become depleted. By signaling the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems, NTs act to regulate cardiac function, breathing, and digestion, along with mood, sleep, concentration, and even weight. When out-of-balance, they can trigger many diverse symptoms, and that's why treating them offers the single most significant improvement in overall patient care. Scientists have estimated that as many as 86% of Americans may experience suboptimal neurotransmitter levels. Stress, poor diet, neurotoxins, genetic predisposition, age, drug use (prescription and recreational), and alcohol and caffeine intake can significantly deplete neurotransmitters levels. Defining a New Approach When it comes to balancing brain chemistry, treatment must be based on an accurate analysis rather than guesswork. For example, some practitioners routinely address weight loss and carbohydrate addiction with glutamine supplementation. But without knowing an individual's precise glutamate levels, this can be risky. Glutamine can convert to glutamate, and exacerbate symptoms such as anxiety, restlessness, and sleep cycle issues. Without NT testing and a comprehensive approach, it's easy to make mistakes. With over 200 plus neurotransmitters, individualexacerbations to the 200th power can occur. Neither the symptom picture nor the individual numbers alone can tell you exactly how to restore balance. What's more, the relationship and ratios between the neurotransmitter levels is key--not just the levels themselves. Assessing and treating the overall picture is not "plug and play." Successful NT treatment has four cornerstones: 1. Proper urine testing with values based on accurate normative data 2. Symptom picture and patient history 3. Customized protocols based on the two cornerstones above and guided by clinical expertise 4. Targeted supplementation Evaluation and treatments based on only one cornerstone would at best be incomplete or imprecise and, at worst, potentially harmful, as can be seen from the following case: Case # 1 At age 86, Tom, a retired engineer, had sleep disturbances, waking every couple of hours throughout the night. Though impressively independent and highly functional for a man his age, he also reported that his memory was slipping a bit. Had I relied solely upon a checklist to determine Tom's protocol, I could have easily been mislead by his symptoms and might have treated the wrong neurotransmitters or under-dosed him. His test results revealed that extremely low levels of norepinephrine (and serotonin) were causing his sleeplessness. If I used a checklist without these test results, I might have treated serotonin and dopamine only--which would have delayed improvement. As a result, he might well have discontinued treatment. Instead, with the lab results pointing to low norepinephrine as the causative factor, I first treated his serotonin levels and then, later, added excitatory support. Soon after, his sleep began to improve. Today, Tom sleeps through the night without waking and reports that his memory has also improved dramatically. Mastering the art of accurately synthesizing and interpreting NT results takes years, and there's no substitute for clinical experience. While some companies and laboratories offer a dozen or more standing protocols for use with certain ranges of test results, in my opinion, these don't accurately capture that complexity. To date, lab personnel do not have any clinical experience to draw from when they create protocols. That's why clinicians should not fully rely upon lab report protocols, although in the absence of proper training, many do. The Neurogistics Brain Wellness Program was created as a service to clinicians to fill this gap. Testing I'm well able to evaluate the merits of different testing options, since over an eight-year period, I've seen over 8,500 results from varied testing modalities. In my view, the 24-hour urine testing was of limited use. When NT testing and evaluation were first developed by some of the luminaries in Neurotransmitter Science, I was fortunate to enter this new field, and work and learn from these pioneers. Urinary neurotransmitter testing provided the first non-invasive and accurate guide to brain chemistry. In a recent Townsend Letter article (October 2006), Julia Ross critiques urine testing as inaccurate. It's certainly true that it took a few years to determine sound reference ranges, but given the rapid evolution in this field, it's a mistake to dismiss the current state-of-the-art testing based on poor results of the past. In recent years, urinary testing has provided a reliable basis for treatment. Today, there are over 500,000 cases collected by three labs that document the validity of both the testing method and corresponding treatment. In her article, Ross also claims that a symptom checklist alone is more reliable, but I strongly disagree. Prior to the advent of urine testing, like the early pioneers in this field, I utilized a checklist for symptoms. But no matter how good the checklist, it wasn't sufficiently accurate to alleviate my patients' symptoms. Although, today, I use a questionnaire to help screen for appropriate tests, as well as to monitor treatment results, there's no point in "guessing" which neurotransmitters require balancing when you can know with scientific certainty. Moreover, an incomplete picture can lead to inaccurate treatment, as in the following case: Case #2 Claire, a young professional in her mid-thirties presented with high anxiety and carbohydrate cravings. Based on a checklist alone, I might have assumed I should give her GABA and perhaps glutamine. Yet the test results revealed a serotonin deficiency, while the patient's GABA level was fine. Why risk creating more glutamate (as can occur with glutamine supplementation) when serotonin was the major culprit? Elevated glutamate can increase anxiety, and in this case, Claire would not have experienced long-term resolution from treating GABA. However, Claire enjoyed full resolution of symptoms with serotonin support. Laboratory results reveal the precise levels and ratios of the range of neurotransmitters. Those can't be derived or quantified from a checklist. In this, neurotransmitter treatment is just like other testing procedures. I would never use a questionnaire to identify a pathogenic bacteria, parasite, or Candida. Instead, I would run a comprehensive diagnostic stool analysis. I would feel remiss in my duty if I did not use the best analytical tools available at any given time, and currently, for NT, the best non-invasive method is urinalysis. Added Benefits of Testing In my opinion, it's not enough to merely identify a deficiency. Test results allow you to quantify a given excretion level. Without that number, you might sub-therapeutically dose a patient and fail to alleviate the symptoms. Conversely, giving too high a dosage can also produce problems. For example, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) teens with drug and/or alcohol problems may have severe serotonin deficiencies. While it's sometimes advisable to put them on a high dosage to bring back their levels, it's important to retest to assure that the levels don't return too quickly. If that were to happen, they could very well flood serotonin receptor sites and experience the same depression and anxiety with which they initially presented. Testing allows you to appropriately adjust treatment. Quantifying levels also indicates when to expect improvement so that patients persist with treatment until they get results. My many Type A clients (such as professional athletes) undertake NT treatment to optimize their brains and their response to stress. Companies rely on our services to improve overall job performance. What's more, testing can reveal incipient problems. I often see executives at the top of their game, just humming, with test results revealing serotonin at a low of 42. If they later experience a major stress, they may find it hard to recuperate. Detecting the problem and treating it early present a significant preventative. With women patients, I often find that after optimizing brain function, they can lower their hormone supplementation to physiological dosages (about 18 mg in the case of progesterone, for example), rather than having to utilize the higher prescription dosages. Why overload yourself with hormones (whether bio-identical or equine), which the body must metabolize to excrete and carry a cancer risk, when through NT balancing, you can use a physiologic dose for its hormonal benefits? Finally, appropriate supplementation is the fifth cornerstone of successful treatment. The supplements we offer target imbalances, while performing the following: * omitting co-factors that interact unfavorably with other practitioner-recommended supplements, * eliminating ingredients potentially problematic for certain health issues (like autism), * avoiding improper combinations of inhibitory and excitatories offered too early in treatment, and * providing ingredient levels appropriate for certain ages or conditions. Balancing Neurotransmitters for Family Health Treating a whole family for neurotransmitter imbalances can improve the relationships and quality of family life. It's fascinating to me how frequently testing reveals that family members share similar neurotransmitter imbalances, although individual symptoms and treatment needs differ. Case #3 When all three members of the Lester family came to me, they were crying out for help, "We are a high-intensity, stressed-out family," Mike, the father, (aged 45) bluntly told me. He had a low libido, while Barbara, the mother, (aged 43) was significantly overweight. Gabe, their son, (aged 13) was defiant, irritable, and unfocused, with behavioral problems experienced at home and in school. Family members were argumentative and irritable with frequent high-decibel explosions. Testing revealed that all three family members had elevated norepinephrine levels, indicating adrenal stress, which fueled anger and impulsivity. Mom's carbohydrate cravings were due to low serotonin levels, while Dad and Gabe had almost identically low dopamine levels. For Dad, low dopamine impacted his libido, while for Gabe, it resulted in ADD. Restoring her serotonin levels helped Mom reduce her carb cravings, and lose weight. Dopamine support (with 1-tyrosine) helped Dad's libido return. Following treatment with 5HTP, phosphatidyl serine, 1-theanine and later 1-tyrosine, Gabe's increased serotonin and reduced norepinephrine levels stabilized his mood, significantly improving his behavior and focus. Even his grades improved. Finally, reducing excitatory neurotransmitter levels for all family members reduced reactivity, impulsivity, and anger. The yelling and screaming stopped. After three months, Barbara Lester reported back to me that, "We're no longer overwhelmed by stress. This is the best thing that ever happened to us. Pam, you helped save my family." With our comprehensive approach, neither practitioners nor patients need ever be on their own. Via clear instructions, supplements, and prompts about when to transition and re-test, patients can become more self-assured. Practitioners are trained and are always in the loop, able to adjust their patients' care and address any problems. "After using the Neurogistics Brain Wellness Program with my clients, I now view Neurotransmitter Balancing as a foundational requirement for most individuals seeking to improve their health and well-being. My practitioner group relies on this program for successful patient outcomes," says William L. Wolcott, recognized worldwide as a leading authority in Metabolic Typing. "Running two multi-functional clinics for twenty-five years, I'd long considered brain chemistry a missing link in integrative health care. In the last year, since I incorporated NT treatment, my patients have finally received treatment for key health problems that have troubled them for years. The results are fantastic, and in some instances, nothing short of miraculous. I've seen undiagnosed bipolars' experience mood stabilization for the first time in their lives." John Franzi, DC, Metabolic Typing Advisor, Australia. Based on my experience and successful outcomes with even difficult cases, I have no doubt that NT balancing works. However, to achieve optimal results, it must be properly implemented. I offer these following guidelines for obtaining a comprehensive approach and ensuring that your patients reap the benefits of Neurotransmitter Balancing: * Determine actual NT levels via an objective measurement; I'm one of 3,500 US practitioners who consider Neurotransmitter Urinanalysis the best noninvasive test currently available. * Beware of simple checklists/questionnaires to replace analytical testing, as they only reveal a partial and often inaccurate view of the brain chemistry. Don't settle for anything less than * precisely customized protocols incorporating complete patient history and NT test results, * solid clinical experience backing all recommendations, * supplementation based on clinically derived protocols, and * patient follow-up to ensure compliance and success. Neurogistics also offers training and support to practitioners worldwide who want to integrate this modality into their practice, making it easier to achieve successful outcomes while learning and growing with this expanding field of treatment. Patients are also welcomed to access our services through our website program. For more information, please visit: www.Neurogistics.com or call (Practitioner) Toll-free: 877-801-8076 (Patient) Toll-free: 888-257-9068 Pam Machemehl Helmly, CN, is a graduate of Texas A & M University with a degree in Scientific Nutrition, She has worked in the health care industry since 1981. In 1997, she began to specialize in NT balancing working with industry pioneers. Since 2005, serving as Chief Science Officer of Neurogistics, Pam has been a leader in the field of changing the brain to improve mood, focus, enhance sports performance, and aid in the treatment of eating disorders. Neurogistics was founded by Pam Machemehl Helmly, CN, and marketing and business expert, Carla Roberts, Neurogistics offers a comprehensive NT balancing program. Our turnkey service includes a synthesis of objective testing results, along with subjective self-reports via a proprietary logic with results customized and reviewed by a clinical team. We provide diagnostics, treatment protocols, practitioner reports, patient reports, prompts to retest, and supplements to encourage compliance, follow-through, troubleshooting, and ongoing maintenance. Notes 1. Elenkov IJ, Wilder RL, Chrousos GP, Vizi ES. The sympathetic nerve--an integrative interface between two supersystems: The brain and the immune system. Pharmacol Rev. 2000 Dec;52(4):595-638. 2. Pliszka SR, McCracken JT, Maas JW. Catecholamines in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Current perspectives. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996 Mar;35(3):264-7. 3. David R. Cross and Karyn B. Purvis. "Neurotransmitter Profiles and Behavior Problems in a Sample of Special Needs Adopted Children." Texas Christian University, March 2005. by Pam Machemehl Helmly, CN COPYRIGHT 2007 The Townsend Letter Group COPYRIGHT 2007 Gale Group

Labels: , , , , , , ,

----- -------- AUTHOR: Biomed Mom TITLE: Neuroarcheology of Childhood Trauma DATE: 6/28/2007 10:21:00 AM ----- BODY:
Child Trauma Academy Home Child Trauma Academy Materials Child Trauma Academy Materials Child Trauma Academy Materials About Child Trauma Academy Child Trauma Academy Services Child Trauma Academy Materials Our Impact Resources & Links Forum The Neuroarcheology of Childhood Maltreatment The Neurodevelopmental Costs of Adverse Childhood Events Bruce D. Perry, M.D., Ph.D. The ChildTrauma Academy www.ChildTrauma.org For: "The Cost of Child Maltreatment: Who Pays? We All Do" (Ed., B. Geffner) Haworth Press July 27, 2000 Introduction Childhood maltreatment has profound impact on the emotional, behavioral, cognitive, social and physical functioning of children. Developmental experiences determine the organizational and functional status of the mature brain and, therefore, adverse events can have a tremendous negative impact on the development of the brain. In turn, these neurodevelopmental effects may result in significant cost to the individual, their family, community and, ultimately, society. In essence, childhood maltreatment alters the potential of a child and, thereby, robs us all. The present chapter will review some of those costs from a neurodevelopmental perspective. The premise is that when the core principles of neurodevelopment are understood, the costs of adverse childhood events and maltreatment become obvious. Following a brief presentation of the key concepts of neurodevelopment, two primary forms of maltreatment will be considered: (1) neglect and (2) traumatic stress. Maltreatment of children often involves both neglect and trauma; a more complete understanding of the complex neurodevelopmental impact of the combination, however, is best understood after presenting the potential effects of each separately. This chapter presents the current articulation of a neurodevelopmental perspective of childhood maltreatment originally outlined in 1994 (Perry. 1994) and further elaborated over the last five years (Perry, Pollard, Blakley, Baker, & Vigilante. 1995) (Perry & Pollard. 1998) This most recent articulation outlines the issue of maltreatment through the lens of developmental neurobiology and coins a descriptive phrase, "neuroarcheology," to capture the impact of adverse events on the developing brain, with the implicit suggestion that experiences leave a 'record' within the matrix of the brain. The nature and location of this record will depend upon the nature of the experience and the time in development when the event took place – much as with the archeological record of the earth. While this phrase may be simplistic to some, it conveys important conceptual principles about the nature of childhood experience which have been lacking all too often in clinical and research formulations regarding maltreatment. Not a single psychometric instrument measuring traumatic or adverse events, for example, uses time of trauma as a meaningful variable despite the fact that it may be the most important determinant of functional outcome following maltreatment. The neuroarcheological perspective on childhood experience, therefore, simply posits that the impact of a childhood event (adverse or positive) will be a reflection of (1) the nature, intensity, pattern and duration of the event and (2) that the resulting strengths (e.g., language) or deficits (e.g., neuropsychiatric symptoms) will be in those functions mediated by the neural systems that are most rapidly organizing (i.e., in the developmental "hot zone") at the time of the experience. Brain Organization and Function The human brain is the remarkable organ that allows us to sense, process, perceive, store and act on information from outside and inside the body to carry out the three prime directives required for the survival of our species: (1) survive, (2) affiliate and mate and then, (3) protect and nurture dependents. In order to carry out these core and overarching responsibilities, thousands of inter-related functions have evolved. In the human brain, structure and function have co-evolved. As we have a hierarchy of increasingly complex functions related to our optimal functioning, our brain has evolved a hierarchical structural organization (see Table 1). This hierarchy starts with the lower, simpler brainstem areas and increases in complexity up through the neocortex (Figure 1). In each of these many areas of the brain are neural systems that mediate our many brain-related functions (Figure1; Table1). The 'lower' parts of the brain (brainstem and midbrain) mediate simpler regulatory functions (e.g., regulation of respiration, heart rate, blood pressure, body temperature) while more complex functions (e.g., language and abstract thinking) are mediated by the more complex neocortical structures of the human brain. This hierarchical structure is the heart of a neuroarcheological understanding of adverse childhood events. This structure becomes the multi-layered soil within which the fossilized evidence of maltreatment can be found – each layer organizing at a different time and each layer reflecting the experiences –good and bad - of that era in the individual's life. Key insights to understanding human functioning, then, will come from understanding neurodevelopment. neuroarcheology1.jpg (33179 bytes) Figure 1: Hierarchical Organization of the Human Brain: The brain can be divided into four interconnected areas: brainstem, diencephalons, limbic and neocortex. The complexity of structure, cellular organization and function increases from the lower, simpler areas such as the brainstem to the most complex, the neocortex. Neurodevelopment Our brain's complex structure is comprised of 100 billion neurons and ten times as many glial cells – all interconnected by trillions of synaptic connections – and communicating in a non-stop, ever-changing dynamic of neurochemical activity. The brain doesn't just pop into existence. This most complex of all biological systems in the known universe is a product of neurodevelopment – a long process orchestrating billions upon billions of complex chemical transactions. It is through these chemical actions that a human being is created. The developing child is a remarkable phenomenon of nature. In a few short years, one single cell – the fertilized egg – becomes a walking, talking, learning, loving, and thinking being. This physical transformation is equivalent to a 6-foot tall, 200 pound man growing to the size of Connecticut in three years. In each of the billions and billions of cells in the body, a single set of genes has been expressed in millions of different combinations with precise timing. Development is a breathtaking orchestration of precision micro-construction that allows the healthy development of a human being. And the most remarkable and complex of all the organs in the human body is the human brain. In order to create the brain, a small set of pre-cursor cells must divide, move, specialize, connect and create specialized neural networks that form functional units. The key processes in neurodevelopment are summarized below. Core Processes of Neurodevelopment 1. Neurogenesis: The brain starts as a few cells present early in the first weeks of life. From a few specialized cells in the unformed brain, come billions of nerve cells and trillions of glia. This, of course, requires that cells be "born." Neurogenesis is the birth of new neurons. The vast majority of neurogenesis takes place in utero during the second and third trimester. At birth, the vast majority of neurons, literally more than 100 billion, used for the remainder of life are present. Few neurons are born after birth, although researchers have demonstrated recently that neurogenesis can and does take place in the mature brain (Gould, Reeves, Graziano, & Gross. 1999). This is a very significant observation and may be one of the important physiological mechanisms responsible for the brain's plasticity (i.e., capacity to restore function) following injury. Despite being present at birth, these neurons have yet to organize into completely functional systems. Many neurons need to mature themselves and undergo a set of processes that create the functional neural networks of the mature brain (Table 2). 2. Migration: Developing neurons move. Often guided by glial cells and a variety of chemical markers (e.g., cellular adhesion molecules, nerve growth factor: NGF), neurons cluster, sort, move and settle into a location in the brain that will be their final "resting" place. It is the fate of some neurons to settle in the brainstem, others in the cortex, for example. More than one half of all neurons are in the cortex. The processes of cortical cell migration and fate mapping are some of the most studied in all of developmental neuroscience (Rakic. 1981) (Rakic. 1996). It is clear that both genetic and environmental factors play important roles in determining a neuron's final location. Migration takes place primarily during the intrauterine and immediate perinatal period but continues throughout childhood and, possibly, to some degree into adult life. A host of intrauterine and perinatal insults – including infection, lack of oxygen, alcohol and various psychotropic drugs can alter migration of neurons and have profound impact on functioning (Perry. 1988). Table 1. A Neuroarcheological Chart of Development: Functional Organization neuroarcheology2.jpg (63967 bytes) 3. Differentiation: Neurons mature. Each of the 100 billion neurons in the brain has the same set of genes, yet each neuron is expressing a unique combination of those genes to create a unique identity. Some neurons are large, with long axons; others short. Neurons can mature to use any of a hundred different neurotransmitters such as norepinephrine, dopamine, serotonin, CRF or substance P. Neurons can have dense dendritic fields receiving input from hundreds of other neurons, while other neurons can have a single linear input from one other neuron. Each of these thousands of differentiating "choices" come as a result of the pattern, intensity and timing of various microenvironmental cues which tell the neuron to turn on some genes and turn off others. Each neuron undergoes a series of "decisions" to determine their final location and specialization. These decisions, again, are a combination of genetic and microenvironmental cues. The further along in development, the more differentiated the neuron, the more sensitive it becomes to the environmental signals. From the intrauterine period through early childhood (and to some degree beyond) neurons are very sensitive to experience-based signals, many of which are mediated by patterned neuronal activity in the neural network in which they reside. Neurons are literally designed to change in response to chemical signals. Therefore, any experience or event that alters these neurochemical or microenvironmental signals during development can change the ways in which certain neurons differentiate, thereby altering the functional capacity of the neural networks in which these neurons reside. 4. Apoptosis: Some developing neurons die. In many areas of the brain, there are more neurons born than are needed for any given function. Many of these neurons are redundant and when unable to adequately "connect" into an active neural network will die (Kuan, Roth, Flavell, & Rakic. 2000). Research in this area suggests that these neurons may play a role in the remarkable flexibility present in the human brain at birth. Depending upon the challenges of the environment and the potential needs of the individual, some neurons will survive while others will not. Again, this process appears to have genetic and environmental determinants. Neurons that make synaptic connections with others and have an adequate level of activation will survive; those cells that have little activity resorb. This is one example of a general principle of activity-dependence ("use it or lose it") that appears to be important in many neural processes related to learning, memory and development. 5. Arborization: As neurons differentiate, they send out tiny fiber-like extensions from their cell body. These dendrites become the receptive area where other neurons connect. It is in this receptive field that dozens to hundreds of other neurons are able to send neurochemical signals to the neuron. The density of these dendritic branches appears to be related to the frequency and intensity of incoming signals. When there is high activity, the dendritic network extends, essentially branching out in the same fashion as a bush may create new branches. This arborization allows the neuron to receive, process and integrate complex patterns of activity that will, in turn, determine its activity. Again, the arborization process appears to be to some degree activity-dependent. The density of the dendritic arborization appears to be related to the complexity and activity of incoming neural activity. In turn, these neural signals are often dependent upon the complexity and activity of the environment of the animal (Diamond, Law, Rhodes, et al. 1966; Greenough, Volkmar, & Juraska. 1973). 6. Synaptogenesis: Developing neurons make connections with each other. The major mechanism for neuron-to-neuron communication is 'receptor-mediated' neurotransmission that takes place at specialized connections between neurons called synapses. At the synapse, the distance between two neurons is very short. A chemical (classified as a neurotransmitter, neuromodulator or neurohormone) is released from the 'presynaptic' neuron and into the extra-cellular space (called the synaptic cleft) and binds to a specialized receptor protein in the membrane of the 'postsynaptic' neuron. By occupying the binding site, the neurotransmitter helps change the shape of this receptor which then catalyzes a secondary set of chemical interactions inside the postsynaptic neuron that create second messengers. The second messengers such as cyclic AMP, inositol phosphate and calcium will then shift the intracellular chemical milieu which may even influence the activity of specific genes. This cascade of intracellular chemical responses allows communication from one neuron to another. A continuous dynamic of synaptic neurotransmission regulates the activity and functional properties of the chains of neurons that allow the brain to do all of its remarkable activities. These neural connections are not random. They are guided by important genetic and environmental cues. In order for our brain to function properly, neurons, during development, need to find and connect with the "right" neurons. During the differentiation process, neurons send fiber-like projections (growth cones) out to make physical contact with other neurons. This process appears to be regulated and guided by certain growth factors and cellular adhesion molecules that attract or repel a specific growth cone to appropriate target neurons. Depending upon a given neuron's specialization, these growth cones will grow (becoming axons) and connect to the dendrites of other cells and create a synapse. During the first eight months of life there is an eight-fold increase in synaptic density while the developing neurons in the brain are "seeking" their appropriate connections (Huttenlocher. 1979) (Huttenlocher. 1994). This explosion of synaptogenesis allows the brain to have the flexibility to organize and function in with a wide range of potential. It is over the next few years, in response to patterned repetitive experiences that these neural connections will be refined and sculpted. 7. Synaptic sculpting: The synapse is a dynamic structure. With ongoing episodic release of neurotransmitter, occupation of receptors, release of growth factors, shifts of ions in and out of cells, laying down of new microtubules and other structural molecules, the synapse is continually changing. A key determinant of change in the synapse appears to be the level of presynaptic activity. When there is a consistent active process of neurotransmitter release, synaptic connections will be strengthened with actual physical changes that make the pre- and postsynaptic neurons come closer and the process of neurotransmission more efficient. When there is little activity, the synaptic connection will literally dissolve. The specific axonal branch to a given neuron will go away. Again, this powerful activity-dependent process appears to be very important for understanding learning, memory and the development. At any given moment – all throughout life – we are making and breaking synaptic connections. For the majority of life we are at equilibrium; the rate of creating new synaptic connections is equal to the rate of resorbing older, unused connections. While somewhat simplistic, it appears that the synaptic sculpting is a "use it or lose it" process. During the first eight months following birth the rate of creating new synapses far outstrips the rate of resorbing unused connections. By age one, however, and from then through early childhood, the rate of resorbing new connections is faster than the rate of creating new synapses. By adolescence, in most cortical areas at least, this process again reaches equilibrium. 8. Myelination: Specialized glial cells wrap around axons and, thereby, create more efficient electrochemical transduction down the neuron. This allows a neural network to function more rapidly and efficiently, thereby allowing more complex functioning (e.g., walking depends upon the myelination of neurons in the spinal cord for efficient, smooth regulation of neuromotor functioning.) The process of myelination begins in the first year of life but continues in many key areas throughout childhood with a final burst of myelination in key cortical areas taking place in adolescence. Table 2: Key Processes in Neurodevelopment neuroarcheology3.jpg (68908 bytes) * This refers to the age at which approximately 10% of this specific function is taking place. In most cases, there is evidence that some of these processes have started to some degree. Almost all of these processes continue in some form throughout life, the table is designed to illustrate the relative importance of childhood for the majority of activity in each of these processes. **These are crude estimates based upon data from multiple sources. The major point it to demonstrate that shifting activity from neurogenesis to myelination. All of the neurodevelopmental processes described above are dependent upon both genetic and environmentally determined microenvironmental cues (e.g., neurotransmitters, neuromodulators, neurohormones, ions, growth factors, cellular adhesion molecules and other morphogens). Disruption of the pattern, timing or intensity of these cues can lead to abnormal neurodevelopment and profound dysfunction. The neuroarcheological perspective suggests that the specific dysfunction will depend upon the timing of the insult (e.g., was the insult in utero during the development of the brainstem or at age two during the active development of the cortex), the nature of the insult (e.g., is there a lack of sensory stimulation from neglect or an abnormal persisting activation of the stress response from trauma?), the pattern of the insult (i.e., is this a discreet single event, a chronic experience with a chaotic pattern or an episodic event with a regular pattern?). While we are only beginning to understand the complexity of neurodevelopment, there are several key principles that emerge from the thousands of studies and years of focused research on these neurodevelopmental processes. These principles, as outlined below, suggest that while the structural organization and functional capabilities of the mature brain can change throughout life, the majority of the key stages of neurodevelopment take place in childhood. The core principles of neurodevelopment that support a neuroarcheological perspective of childhood adverse events are summarized below. Core Principles of Neurodevelopment 1. Nature and nurture: For too many years, any conceptual approach to human behavior has been tainted by the nature versus nurture debate. Do genes cause human behavior or is human behavior a product of learning, education and experience? Ultimately, this debate polarizes and distracts from more complex understandings of human functioning. Genes are designed to work in an environment. Genes are expressed by microenvironmental cues, which, in turn, are influenced by the experiences of the individual. How an individual functions within an environment, then, is dependent upon the expression of a unique combination of genes available to the human species. We don't have the genes to make wings. And what we become depends upon how experiences shape the expression – or not - of specific genes we do have. We do have the genes to make forty sounds – and we can have the experiences that turn this genetically determined capacity into a powerful, transforming tool – language. Yet, there are many sad examples of cruel experiments of humanity, where a young child was raised in an environment deprived of language. This child, despite the genetic potential to speak and think and feel in complex humane ways, did not express that potential fully. Genetic potential without appropriately timed experiences can remain unexpressed. Nature and nurture – we are nothing without both; we require both and we are products of both. The influence of gene-driven processes, however, shifts during development. In the just fertilized ovum, all of the chemical processes that are driving development are very dependent upon a genetically determined sequence of molecular events. By birth, however, the brain has developed to the point where environmental cues mediated by the senses play a major role in determining how neurons will differentiate, sprout dendrites, form and maintain synaptic connections and create the final neural networks that convey functionality. By adolescence, the majority of the changes that are taking place in the brain of that child are determined by experience, not genetics. The languages, beliefs, cultural practices, and complex cognitive and emotional functioning (e.g., self esteem) by this age are primarily experience-based. 2. Sequential Developmental: The brain develops in a sequential and hierarchical fashion; organizing itself from least (brainstem) to most complex (limbic, cortical areas). These different areas develop, organize and become fully functional at different times during childhood. At birth, for example, the brainstem areas responsible for regulating cardiovascular and respiratory function must be intact for the infant to survive, and any malfunction is immediately observable. In contrast, the cortical areas responsible for abstract cognition have years before they will be 'needed' or fully functional. This means that each brain area will have its own timetable for development. The neurodevelopmental processes described above will be most active in different brain areas at different times and will, therefore, either require (critical periods) or be sensitive to (sensitive periods) organizing experiences (and the neurotrophic cues related to these experiences). The neurons for the brainstem have to migrate, differentiate and connect, for example, before the neurons for the cortex. The implications of this for a neuroarcheological formulation are profound. Disruptions of experience-dependent neurochemical signals during these periods may lead to major abnormalities or deficits in neurodevelopment. Disruption of critical neurodevelopmental cues can result from 1) lack of sensory experience during sensitive periods (e.g., neglect) or 2) atypical or abnormal patterns of necessary cues due to extremes of experience (e.g., traumatic stress, see below). Insults during the intrauterine period, for example, will more likely influence the rapidly organizing brainstem systems as opposed to the more slowly organizing cortical areas. The symptoms from the intrauterine disruption will alter functions mediated by the brainstem and could include sensory integration problems, hyper-reactivity, poor state regulation (e.g., sleep, feeding, self-soothing), tactile defensiveness and altered regulation of core neurophysiological functions such as respiration, cardiovascular and temperature regulation. This does not mean that neocortical systems are unaffected by disrupting the development of the brainstem. Indeed, one of the most important aspects of the sequential development is that important organizing signals for any given brain area or system (e.g., patterns of neural activity, neurotransmitters acting as morphogens) come from previously organized brain areas or systems. Due to the sequential development of the brain, disruptions of normal developmental processes early in life (e.g., during the perinatal period) that alter development of the brainstem or diencephalon will necessarily alter the development of limbic and cortical areas. This is so because many of the organizing cues for normal limbic and neocortical organization originate in the lower brain areas. Any developmental insult can have a cascade effect on the development of all "downstream" brain areas (and functions) that will receive input from the effected neural system. 3. Activity-dependent neurodevelopment: The brain organizes in a use-dependent fashion. As described above, many of the key processes in neurodevelopment are activity dependent. In the developing brain, undifferentiated neural systems are critically dependent upon sets of environmental and micro-environmental cues (e.g., neurotransmitters, cellular adhesion molecules, neurohormones, amino acids, ions) in order for them to appropriately organize from their undifferentiated, immature forms (Lauder. 1988; Perry. 1994) (Perry & Pollard. 1998). Lack, or disruption, of these critical cues can alter the neurodevelopmental processes of neurogenesis, migration, differentiation, synaptogenesis - all of which can contribute to malorganization and diminished functional capabilities in the specific neural system where development has been disrupted. This is the core of a neuroarcheological perspective on dysfunction related adverse childhood events (Perry. 1994) (Perry & Pollard. 1998; Perry. 1998). These molecular cues that guide development are dependent upon the experiences of the developing child. The quantity, pattern of activity and nature of these neurochemical and neurotrophic factors depends upon the presence and the nature of the total sensory experience of the child. When the child has adverse experiences – loss, threat, neglect, and injury – there can be disruptions of neurodevelopment that will result in neural organization that can lead to compromised functioning throughout life (see Neglect section, below). A neuroarcheological perspective would predict that the dysfunction resulting from a specific adverse event is related to the disrupted (or altered) development of the neural system that is, during the adverse event, most rapidly developing. The degree of disruption is related to the rate of change in the respective neural system. The already organized and functioning neural system is less vulnerable to a developmental insult than the rapidly changing, energy-hungry and microenvironmental cue-sensitive developing system. This is so because of a principle called biological relativity. In any dynamic system, the impact of an event or experience (disruptive or positive) is greatest on the most actively changing or dynamic parts of that system. The power of any experience, therefore, is greatest during the most rapid phases of development. Events taking place during a neural system's most active phase of organization will have more impact than events after the system has organized. 4. Windows of Opportunity/Windows of Vulnerability: The sequential development of the brain and the activity-dependence of many key aspects of neurodevelopment suggest that there must be times during development when a given developing neural system is more sensitive to experience than others (Table 3). In healthy development, that sensitivity allows the brain to rapidly and efficiently organize in response to the unique demands of a given environment to express from its broad genetic potential those characteristics which best fit that child's world. If the child speaks Japanese as opposed to English, for example, or if this child will live in the plains of Africa or the tundra of the Yukon, different genes can be expressed, different neural networks can be organized from that child's potential to best fit that family, culture and environment. We all are aware of how rapidly young children can learn language, develop new behaviors and master new tasks. The very same neurodevelopmental sensitivity that allows amazing developmental advances in response to predictable, nurturing, repetitive and enriching experiences make the developing child vulnerable to adverse experiences. Sensitive periods are different for each brain area and neural system, and therefore, for different functions. The sequential development of the brain and the sequential unfolding of the genetic map for development mean that the sensitive periods for neural system (and the functions they mediate) will be when that system is in the developmental 'hot zone' – when that area is most actively organizing. The brainstem must organize key systems by birth; therefore, the sensitive period for those brainstem-mediated functions is during the prenatal period. The neocortex, in contrast, has systems and functions organizing throughout childhood and into adult life. The sensitive periods for these cortically mediated functions are likely to be very long. With an understanding of the shifting vulnerability of the developing brain to experience, a neuroarcheological perspective becomes apparent. If there are disrupting adverse events during development, they will be mirrored by a matched dysfunctional development in the neural systems whose functioning the adverse experience most altered during the event. If the disruption were the absence of light during the first year of life – the systems most altered would be related to vision. If the disruption activates the stress response, the disruption will be in the neural systems mediating the stress response. The severity and chronicity of the specific dysfunction will be related to the vulnerability of the system affected. Adverse experiences influence the mature brain but in the developing brain, adverse experiences literally play a role in organizing neural systems. It is much easier to influence the functioning of a developing system than to reorganize and alter the functioning of a developed system. Adverse childhood events, therefore, can alter the organization of developing neural systems in ways that create a lifetime of vulnerability. Table 3: Shifting Developmental Activity across Brain Regions neuroarcheology4.jpg (35426 bytes) The simple and unavoidable conclusion of these neurodevelopmental principles is that the organizing, sensitive brain of an infant or young children is more malleable to experience than a mature brain. While experience may alter the behavior of an adult, experience literally provides the organizing framework for an infant and child. Because the brain is most plastic (receptive to environmental input) in early childhood, the child is most vulnerable to variance of experience during this time. In the second half of this chapter two primary forms of extreme childhood adverse experience will be discussed in context of the neuroarcheological perspective of adverse childhood events. The Neurodevelopmental Impact of Neglect in Childhood Neglect is the absence of critical organizing experiences at key times during development. Despite its obvious importance in understanding child maltreatment, neglect has been understudied. Indeed, deprivation of critical experiences during development may be the most destructive yet the least understood area of child maltreatment. There are several reasons for this. The most obvious is that neglect is difficult to "see." Unlike a broken bone, maldevelopment of neural systems mediating empathy, for example, resulting from emotional neglect during infancy, is not readily observable. Another important, yet poorly appreciated, aspect of neglect is the issue of timing. The needs of the child shift during development; therefore, what may be neglectful at one age is not at another. The very same experience that is essential for life at one stage of life may be of little significance or even inappropriate at another age. We would all question the mother who held, rocked and breastfed her pubescent child. Touch, for example, is essential during infancy. The untouched newborn may literally die; in Spitz' landmark studies, the mortality rates in the institutionalized infants was near thirty percent (Spitz. 1945; Spitz. 1946). If one doesn't touch an adolescent for weeks, however, no significant adverse effects will result. Creating standardized protocols, procedures and "measures" of neglect, therefore, are significantly confounded by the shifting developmental needs and demands of childhood. Finally, neglect is understudied because it is very difficult to find large populations of humans where specific and controlled neglectful experiences have been well documented. In some cases, these cruel experiments of humanity have provided unique and promising insights (see below). In general, however, there will never be – and there never should be – the opportunity to study neglect in humans with the rigor that can be applied in animal models. With these limitations, however, what we do know about neglect during early childhood supports a neuroarcheological view of adverse childhood experience. The earlier and more pervasive the neglect is, the more devastating the developmental problems for the child. Indeed, a chaotic, inattentive and ignorant caregiver can produce pervasive developmental delay (PDD; (Anonymous. 1994)) in a young child (Rutter, Andersen-Wood, Beckett, et al. 1999). Yet the very same inattention for the same duration if the child is ten will have very different and less severe impact than inattention during the first years of life. There are two main sources of insight to childhood neglect. The first is the indirect but more rigorous animal studies and the second is a growing number of descriptive reports with severely neglected children. Environmental Manipulation and Neurodevelopment: Animal Studies Some of the most important studies in developmental neurosciences in the last century have been focusing on various aspects of experience and extreme sensory experience models. Indeed, the Nobel Prize was awarded to Hubel and Weisel for their landmark studies on development of the visual system using sensory deprivation techniques (Hubel & Wiesel. 1963). In hundreds of other studies, extremes of sensory deprivation (Hubel & Wiesel. 1970; Greenough, Volkmar, & Juraska. 1973) or sensory enrichment (Greenough & Volkmar. 1973; Diamond, Krech, & Rosenzweig. 1964; Diamond, Law, Rhodes, et al. 1966) have been studied. These include disruptions of visual stimuli (Coleman & Riesen. 1968), environmental enrichment (Altman & Das. 1964; Cummins & Livesey. 1979), touch (Ebinger. 1974; Rutledge, Wright, & Duncan. 1974), and other factors that alter the typical experiences of development (Uno, Tarara, Else, & et.al. 1989; Plotsky & Meaney. 1993; Meaney, Aitken, van Berkal, Bhatnagar, & Sapolsky. 1988). These findings generally demonstrate that the brains of animals reared in enriched environments are larger, more complex and functional more flexible than those raised under deprivation conditions. Diamond's work, for example, examining the relationships between experience and brain cytoarchitecture have demonstrated a relationship between density of dendritic branching and the complexity of an environment (for a good review of this and related data see (Diamond & Hopson. 1998)). Others have shown that rats raised in environmentally enriched environments have higher density of various neuronal and glial microstructures, including a 30% higher synaptic density in cortex compared to rats raised in an environmentally deprived setting (Bennett, Diamond, Krech, & Rosenzweig. 1964; Altman & Das. 1964). Animals raised in the wild have from 15 to 30% larger brain mass than their offspring who are domestically reared (Darwin. 1868; Rohrs. 1955; Rohrs & Ebinger. 1978; Rehkamper, Haase, & Frahm. 1988). Animal studies suggest that critical periods exist during which specific sensory experience was required for optimal organization and development of the part of the brain mediating a specific function (e.g., visual input during the development of the visual cortex). While these phenomena have been examined in great detail for the primary sensory modalities in animals, few studies have examined the issues of critical or sensitive periods in humans. What evidence there is would suggest that humans tend to have longer periods of sensitivity and that the concept of critical period may not be useful in humans. It is plausible, however, that abnormal micro-environmental cues and atypical patterns of neural activity during sensitive periods in humans could result in malorganization and compromised function in a host of brain-mediated functions. Indeed, altered emotional, behavioral, cognitive, social and physical functioning has been demonstrated in humans following specific types of neglect. The majority of this information comes from the clinical rather than the experimental disciplines. The Impact of Neglect in Early Childhood: Clinical Findings Over the last sixty years, many case reports, case series and descriptive studies have been conducted with children neglected in early childhood. The majority of these studies have focused on institutionalized children. As early as 1833, with the famous Kaspar Hauser, feral children had been described (Heidenreich. 1834). Hauser was abandoned as a young child and raised from early childhood (likely around age two) until seventeen in a dungeon, experiencing relative sensory, emotional and cognitive neglect. His emotional, behavioral and cognitive functioning was, as one might expect, very primitive and delayed. At autopsy, Hauser's brain was noted to have a small cerebrum (cortex) with few and non-distinct cortical gyri. These findings are consistent with cortical atrophy (or underdevelopment), a condition we have reported in children following severe total global neglect in childhood (Perry & Pollard. 1997). In the early forties, Spitz described the impact of neglectful caregiving on children in foundling homes (orphanages). Most significant, he was able to demonstrate that children raised in fostered placements with more attentive and nurturing caregiving had superior physical, emotional and cognitive outcomes (Spitz. 1945; Spitz. 1946). Some of the most powerful clinical examples of this phenomenon are related to profound neglect experiences early in life. In a landmark report of children raised in a Lebanese orphanage, the Creche, Dennis (1973) described a series of findings supporting a neuroarcheological model of maltreatment. These children were raised in an institutional environment devoid of individual attention, cognitive stimulation, emotional affection or other enrichment. Prior to 1956 all of these children remained at the orphanage until age six, at which time they were transferred to another institution. Evaluation of these children at age 16 demonstrated a mean IQ of approximately 50. When adoption became common, children adopted prior to age 2 had a mean IQ of 100 by adolescence while children adopted between ages 2 and 6 had IQ values of approximately 80 (Dennis. 1973). This graded recovery reflected the neuroarcheological impact of neglect. A number of similar studies of children adopted from neglectful settings demonstrate this general principle. The older a child was at time of adoption, (i.e., the longer the child spent in the neglectful environment) the more pervasive and resistant to recovery were the deficits. Money and Annecillo (1976) reported the impact of change in placement on children with psychosocial dwarfism (failure to thrive). In this preliminary study, 12 of 16 children removed from neglectful homes recorded remarkable increases in IQ and other aspects of emotional and behavioral functioning. Furthermore, they reported that the longer the child was out of the abusive home the higher the increase in IQ. In some cases IQ increased by 55 points (Money & Annecillo. 1976). A more recent report on a group of 111 Romanian orphans (Rutter & English and Romanian Adoptees study team. 1998; Rutter, Andersen-Wood, Beckett, et al. 1999) adopted prior to age two from very emotionally and physically depriving institutional settings demonstrate similar findings. Approximately one half of the children were adopted prior to age six months and the other half between six months and 2 years old. At the time of adoption, these children had significant delays. Four years after being placed in stable and enriching environments, these children were re-evaluated. While both groups improved, the group adopted at a younger age had a significantly greater improvement in all domains. These observations are consistent with the experiences of our clinic research group working with maltreated children. Over the last ten-year we have worked with more than 1000 children neglected in some fashion. We have recorded increases in IQ of over 40 points in more than 60 children following removal from neglectful environments and placed in consistent, predictable, nurturing, safe and enriching placements (Perry et al., in preparation). In addition, in a study of more than 200 children under the age of 6 removed from parental care following abuse and neglect we demonstrated significant developmental delays in more than 85% of the children. The severity of these developmental problems increased with age, suggesting, again, that the longer the child was in the adverse environment - the earlier and more pervasive the neglect - the more indelible and pervasive the deficits. The impact of deprivation can be approximated by sensory chaos. Indeed, sensory deprivation is much less clinically significant than sensory chaos. The vast majority of children suffering from neglect do so because their experiences are chaotic, dysynchronous, inconsistent and episodic rather than consistent, predictable and continuous. The organizing brain requires patterns of sensory experience to create patterns of neural activity that, in turn, play a role in guiding the various neurodevelopmental processes involved in healthy development. When experience is chaotic or sensory patterns are not consistent and predictable, the organizing systems in the brain reflect this chaos and, typically, organize in ways that result in dysregulation and dysynchronous. Imagine trying to learn a language if you only heard random words without the context, grammar and syntax of the language (i.e., the patterns of use). Even if you heard and perceived all words, you could not develop language. Random exposure to words absent an organizing pattern leads to abnormal development of speech and language. Our clinical group has evaluated many children capable of parroting advertising phrases from television but incapable of simple verbal communication. This requirement for consistent, repetitive and patterned stimuli holds for all experience – cognitive, emotional, social and physical. Repetitive, patterned, consistent experience allows the brain to create an internal representation of the external world. A child growing up in the midst of chaos and unpredictability will develop neural systems and functional capabilities that reflect this disorganization. The Impact of Neglect in Early Childhood: Neurobiological Findings All of these reported developmental problems – language, fine and large motor delays, impulsivity, disorganized attachment, dysphoria, attention and hyperactivity, and a host of others described in these neglected children – are caused by abnormalities in the brain. Despite this obvious statement, very few studies have examined directly any aspect of neurobiology in neglected children. The reasons include a lack of capacity, until the recent past, to examine the brain in any non-invasive fashion. Our group has examined various aspects of neurodevelopment in neglected children (Perry & Pollard. 1997). Neglect was considered global neglect when a history of relative sensory deprivation in more than one domain was obtained (e.g., minimal exposure to language, touch and social interactions). Chaotic neglect is far more common and was considered present if history was obtained that was consistent with physical, emotional, social or cognitive neglect. When possible history was obtained from multiple sources (e.g., investigating CPS workers, family, police). The neglected children (n= 122) were divided into four groups: Global Neglect (GN; n=40); Global Neglect with Prenatal Drug Exposure (GN+PND; n=18); Chaotic Neglect (CN; n=36); Chaotic Neglect with Prenatal Drug Exposure (CN+PND; n=28). Measures of growth were compared across group and compared to standard norms developed and used in all major pediatric settings. Dramatic differences from the norm were observed in FOC (the frontal-occipital circumference, a measure of head size and in young children a reasonable measure of brain size). In the globally neglected children the lower FOC values suggested abnormal brain growth. For these globally neglected children the group mean was below the 8th percentile. In contrast, the chaotically neglected children did not demonstrate this marked group difference in FOC. Furthermore in cases where MRI or CT scans were available, neuroradiologists interpreted 11 of 17 scans as abnormal from the children with global neglect (64.7 %) and only 3 of 26 scans abnormal from the children with chaotic neglect (11.5 %). The majority of the readings were "enlarged ventricles" or "cortical atrophy." While the actual size of the brain in chaotically neglected children did not appear to be different from norms, it is reasonable to hypothesize that organizational abnormalities exist and that with function MRI studies these abnormalities will be more readily detected. These findings strongly suggest that when early life neglect is characterized by decreased sensory input (e.g., relative poverty of words, touch and social interactions) there will be a similar effect on human brain growth as in other mammalian species. The human cortex grows in size, develops complexity, makes synaptic connections and modifies as a function of the quality and quantity of sensory experience. Lack of type and quantity of sensory-motor and cognitive experiences lead to underdevelopment of the cortex – in rats, non-human primates and humans. Studies from other groups are beginning to report similar altered neurodevelopment in neglected children. In the study of Romanian orphans described above, the 38 % had FOC values below the third percentile (greater than 2 SD from the norm) at the time of adoption. In the group adopted after six months, fewer than 3 % and the group adopted after six months 13 % had persistently low FOCs four years later (Rutter & English and Romanian Adoptees study team. 1998; O'Connor, Rutter, & English and Romanian Adoptees study team. 2000). Strathearn (Strathearn et al., submitted) has followed extremely low birth weight infants and shown that when these infants end up in neglectful homes they have a significantly smaller head circumference at 2 and 4 years, but not at birth. This is despite having no significant difference in other growth parameters. Finally in a related population, maltreated children and adolescents with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), De Bellis and colleagues found that subject children have significantly smaller intracranial and cerebral volumes than matched controls on MRI scan. Brain volume in these children correlated "robustly and positively" with the age of onset of PTSD trauma, and negatively with the duration of abuse, suggesting that traumatic childhood experiences may adversely affect brain development. Specific brain areas were affected differentially, in reflection of their importance in the stress response, further support of a neuroarcheological formulation of adverse childhood experience (De Bellis, Keshavan, Clark, et al. 1999). While deprivations and lack of specific sensory experiences are common in the maltreated child, the traumatized child experiences developmental insults related to discrete patterns of over-activation of neurochemical cues. Rather than a deprivation of sensory stimuli, the traumatized child experiences over-activation of important neural systems during sensitive periods of development. The Neurodevelopmental Impact of Traumatic Stress in Childhood Each year in United States more than five million children are exposed to some form of extreme traumatic stressor. These traumatic events include natural disasters (e.g., tornadoes, floods, hurricanes), motor vehicle accidents, life threatening illness and associated painful medical procedures (e.g., severe burns, cancer), physical abuse, sexual assault, witnessing domestic or community violence, kidnapping and sudden death of a parent, among others (Pfefferbaum. 1997; Anonymous. 1998). These events, posing an actual or perceived threat to the individual, activate a stress response. During the traumatic event, the child's brain mediates the adaptive response. Brainstem and diencehpalic stress-mediating neural systems are activated. These systems include the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, central nervous system (CNS) noradrenergic (NA), dopaminergic (DA) systems and associated CNS and peripheral systems that provide the adaptive emotional, behavioral, cognitive and physiological changes necessary for survival (Perry. 1994; Perry & Pollard. 1998). Individual neurobiological responses during traumatic stress are heterogeneous (Perry, Pollard, Blakley, Baker, & Vigilante. 1995). The specific nature of a child's responses to a given traumatic event may vary with the nature, duration and the pattern of traumatic stressor and the child's constitutional characteristics (e.g., genetic predisposition, age, gender, history of previous stress exposure, presence of attenuating factors such as supportive caregivers). Whatever the individual response, however, the extreme nature of the external threat is matched by an extreme and persisting internal activation of the neurophysiological systems mediating the stress response and their associated functions (Perry, Pollard, Blakley, Baker, & Vigilante. 1995; Perry & Pollard. 1998). As described above, neural systems respond to prolonged, repetitive activation by altering their neurochemical and sometimes, microarchitectural (e.g., synaptic sculpting) organization and functioning. This is no different for the neural systems mediating the stress response. Following any traumatic event children will likely experience some persisting emotional, behavioral, cognitive and physiological signs and symptoms related to the, sometimes temporary, shifts in the activity of these neural systems originating in the brainstem and diencephalon. In general, the longer the activation of the stress-response systems (i.e., the more intense and prolonged the traumatic event), the more likely there will be a 'use-dependent' change in these neural systems (for review see (Perry & Pollard. 1998)). In some cases, then, the stress-response systems do not return to the pre-event homeostasis. In these cases, the signs and symptoms become so severe, persisting and disruptive that they reach the level of a clinical disorder (Perry. 1998). In a new context and in the absence of any true external threat, the abnormal persistence of a once adaptive response becomes maladaptive. Post traumatic stress-related clinical syndromes Post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a clinical syndrome that may develop following extreme traumatic stress (DSM IV) (Anonymous. 1994). Like all other DSM IV diagnoses, it is likely that heterogeneous pathophysiologies underlie the cluster of diagnostic signs and symptoms labeled PTSD. There are six diagnostic criteria for PTSD: 1) extreme traumatic stress accompanied by intense fear, horror or disorganized behavior; 2) persistent re-experiencing of the traumatic event such as repetitive play or recurring intrusive thoughts; 3) avoidance of cues associated with the trauma or emotional numbing; 4) persistent physiological hyper-reactivity or arousal; 5) signs and symptoms present for more than one month following the traumatic event and 6) clinically significant disturbance in functioning. Posttraumatic stress disorder has been studied primarily in adult populations, most commonly combat veterans and victims of sexual assault. Despite high numbers of traumatized children, the clinical phenomenology, treatment and neurophysiological correlates of childhood PTSD remain under studied. The clinical phenomenology of trauma-related neuropsychiatric sequelae is poorly characterized (Terr. 1991; Mulder, Fergusson, Beautrais, & Joyce. 1998). Most of the studies of PTSD have been following single discreet trauma (e.g., a shooting). The least characterized populations are very young children and children with multiple or chronic traumatic events. Clinical presentations If during development, this stress response apparatus are required to be persistently active, the stress response apparatus in the central nervous system will develop in response to constant threat. These stress-response neural systems (and all functions they mediate – including sympathetic-parasympathetic tone, level of vigilance, regulation of mood, attention and sleep) will be poorly regulated, often overactive and hypersensitive. It is highly adaptive for a child growing up in a violent, chaotic environment to be hypersensitive to external stimuli, to be hypervigilant, and to be in a persistent stress-response state. It is important to realize that children exposed to traumatic stress during development literally organize their neural systems to adapt to this kind of environment. In contrast, an adult with no previous traumatic stress can develop PTSD. The cardiovascular reactivity and physiological hypersensitivity that the adult develops, however, is cue specific. This means that they will demonstrate increased heart rate, startle response and other neurophysiological symptoms when exposed to a cue from the original trauma (e.g., the Vietnam vet hearing a helicopter). In contrast, young children will develop a generalized physiological hyper-reactivity and hypersensitivity to all cues that activate the stress response apparatus. This generalized change results when the traumatic stress literally provides the organizing cues for their developing stress response neurobiology (Perry. 1999). Clinically, this is very easily seen in children who are exposed to chronic neurodevelopmental trauma. These children are frequently diagnosed as having attention deficit disorder (ADD-H) with hyperactivity (Haddad & Garralda. 1992). This is somewhat misleading, however. These children are hypervigilant; they do not have a core abnormality of their capacity to attend to a given task. These children have behavioral impulsivity, and cognitive distortions all of which result from a use-dependent organization of the brain (Perry, Pollard, Blakley, Baker, & Vigilante. 1995). During development, these children spent so much time in a low-level state of fear (mediated by brainstem and diencehpalic areas) that they consistently were focusing on non-verbal but not verbal cues. In our clinical population, children raised in chronically traumatic environments demonstrate a prominent V-P split on IQ testing (n = 108; WISC Verbal = 8. 2; WISC Performance = 10.4, Perry et al., in preparation). Often these children are labeled as learning disabled. We have seen these V-P splits in children in the juvenile justice system, child protective system and in the specialized clinical populations referred to our ChildTrauma clinic. These children are also characterized by persisting physiological hyperarousal and hyperactivity (Perry, Pollard, Baker, Sturges, Vigilante, & Blakley. 1995; Perry. 1994; Perry. 2000). These children are observed to have increased muscle tone, frequently a low grade increase in temperature, an increased startle response, profound sleep disturbances, affect regulation problems and anxiety (Kaufman. 1991; Ornitz & Pynoos. 1989; Perry. 2000). In addition, our studies indicate that a significant portion of these children have abnormalities in cardiovascular regulation (Perry, Pollard, Baker, Sturges, Vigilante, & Blakley. 1995; Perry. 2000). All of these symptoms are the result of a use-dependent organization of the brain stem nuclei involved in the stress response apparatus. Children with PTSD may present with a combination of problems including impulsivity, distractibility and attention problems (due to hypervigilance), dysphoria, emotional numbing, social avoidance, dissociation, sleep problems, aggressive (often re-enactment) play, school failure and regressed or delayed development. In most studies examining the development of PTSD following a given traumatic experience, twice as many children suffer from significant post-traumatic signs or symptoms (PTSS) but lack all of the criteria necessary for the diagnosis of PTSD (Friedrich. 1998). In these cases, the clinician may identify the trauma-related symptom as being part of another neuropsychiatric syndrome. The clinician is often unaware of ongoing traumatic stressors (e.g., domestic or community violence) or the family makes no association between the present symptoms and past events (e.g., car accident, death of a relative, exposure to violence) and may provide no relevant history to aid the clinician in the differential. As a result, PTSD is frequently misdiagnosed and PTSS are under recognized. Children with PTSD as a primary diagnosis are often labeled with Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity (ADHD), major depression, oppositional-defiant disorder, conduct disorder, separation anxiety or specific phobia. Ackerman and colleagues examined the prevalence of PTSD and other neuropsychiatric disorders in 204 abused children (ages 7 to 13) (Ackerman, Newton, McPHerson, Jones, & Dykman. 1998). Thirty four percent of these children met criteria for PTSD. Over fifty percent of the children in this study suffering both physical and sexual abuse had PTSD. Using structured diagnostic interview, the majority of these children met diagnostic criteria for three or more Axis I diagnoses in addition to PTSD. Indeed, only 6 of 204 children met criteria for only PTSD. The broad co-morbidity reported in this study echoes previous studies. Incidence and prevalence Children exposed to various traumatic events have much higher incidence (from 15 to 90+ %) and prevalence rates than the general population (Pfefferbaum. 1997). Furthermore, the younger a child is the more vulnerable they appear to be for the development of trauma-related symptoms. The percentage of children developing PTSD following a traumatic event is significantly higher than the percentage of adults developing PTSD following a similar traumatic stress. Several studies published in 1998 confirm previous reports of high prevalence rates for PTSD in child and adolescent populations. Thirty five percent of a sample of adolescents diagnosed with cancer met criteria for lifetime PTSD (Pelcovitz, Kaplan, Goldenberg, Mandel, Lehane, & Guarrera. 1994); 15 % of children surviving cancer had moderate to severe PTSS (Stuber, Kazak, Meeske, et al. 1997); 93 % of a sample of children witnessing domestic violence had PTSD (Kilpatrick & Williams. 1998); over 80 % of the Kuwaiti children exposed to the violence of the Gulf Crisis had PTSS (Hadi & Llabre. 1998); 73 % of juvenile male rape victims develop PTSD (Ruchkin, Eisemann, & Hagglof. 1998); 34 % of a sample of children experiencing sexual or physical abuse and 58 % of children experiencing both physical and sexual abuse all met criteria for PTSD (Ackerman, Newton, McPHerson, Jones, & Dykman. 1998). In all of these studies, clinically significant symptoms, though not full PTSD, were observed in essentially all of the children or adolescents following the traumatic experiences. Vulnerability and resilience Not all children exposed to traumatic events develop PTSD. A major research focus has been identifying factors (mediating factors) that are associated with increased (vulnerability) or decreased (resilience) risk for developing PTSD following exposure to traumatic stress (Kilpatrick & Williams. 1998). Factors previously demonstrated to be related to risk can be summarized in these broad categories: 1) characteristics of the child (e.g., subjective perception of threat to life or limb, history of previous traumatic exposures, coping style, general level of anxiety, gender, age); 2) characteristics of the event (e.g., nature of the event, direct physical harm, proximity to threat, pattern and duration); 3) characteristics of family/social system (e.g., supportive, calm, nurturing vs. chaotic, distant, absent, anxious) (Briggs & Joyce. 1997; Stuber, Kazak, Meeske, et al. 1997; Winje & Ulvik. 1998). Each of these mediating factors can be related to the degree to which they either prolong or attenuate the child's stress-response activation resulting from the traumatic experience. Factors that increase stress-related reactivity (e.g., family chaos) will make children more vulnerable while factors that provide structure, predictability, nurturing and sense of safety will decrease vulnerability. Persistently activated stress-response neurophysiology in the dependent, fearful child will predispose to a 'use-dependent' changes in the neural systems mediated the stress response, thereby resulting in post-traumatic stress symptoms (see Table 4). Table 4. Post-traumatic Stress Disorder: Risk and Attenuating Factors neuroarcheology5.jpg (94803 bytes) Long-term costs of childhood trauma PTSD is a chronic disorder. Untreated, PTSS and PTSD remit at a very low rate. Indeed the residual emotional, behavioral, cognitive and social sequelae of childhood trauma persist and appear to contribute to a host of neuropsychiatric problems throughout life (Fergusson & Horwood. 1998) including attachment problems (Bell & Belicki. 1998; Alexander, Anderson, Brand, Schaeffer, Grelling, & Kretz. 1998), eating disorders (Rorty & Yager. 1996), depression (Winje & Ulvik. 1998; Fergusson & Horwood. 1998), suicidal behavior (Molnar, Shade, Kral, Booth, & Watters. 1998), anxiety (Fergusson & Horwood. 1998), alcoholism (Fergusson & Horwood. 1998; Epstein, Saunders, Kilpatrick, & Resnick. 1998), violent behavior (O'Keefe. 1995), mood disorders (Kaufman. 1991) and, of course, PTSD (Ford & Kidd. 1998; Schaaf & McCanne. 1998). Childhood trauma impacts other aspects of physical health throughout life, as well (Hertzman & Wiens. 1996; Orr, Lasko, Metzger, Berry, Ahern, & Pitman. 1998; Felliti, Anda, Nordenberg, et al. 1998). Adults victimized by sexual abuse in childhood are more likely to have difficulty in childbirth, a variety of gastrointestinal and gynecological disorders and other somatic problems such as chronic pain, headaches and fatigue (Rhodes & Hutchinson. 1994). The Adverse Childhood Experiences study (Felliti, Anda, Nordenberg, et al. 1998) examined exposure to seven categories of adverse events during childhood (e.g., sexual abuse, physical abuse, witnessing domestic violence: events associated with increase risk for PTSD). This study found a graded relationship between the number of adverse events in childhood and the adult health and disease outcomes examined (e.g., heart disease, cancer, chronic lung disease, and various risk behaviors). With four or more adverse childhood events, the risk for various medical conditions increased 4- to 12-fold. Clearly studies of this sort will help clarify the true costs of childhood maltreatment. Summary and Future Directions The remarkable property of the human brain, unlike any other animal species, is that it has the capacity to take the accumulated experience of thousands of previous generations and absorb it within one lifetime. This capability is endowed by the design of our neural systems. Neurons and neural systems are designed to change in response to microenvironmental events. In turn, our experiences influence the pattern and nature of these microenvironmental signals, allowing neural systems to create a biological record of our lives. The brain, then, becomes an historical organ. In its organization and functioning are memorialized our accumulated, synthesized and transformed experiences. And there is no greater period of sensitivity to experience than when the brain is developing. Indeed, as described above, the neuroarcheological record of maltreatment has pervasive and chronic impact on the child. An event that lasts a few months in infancy can rob a child's potential for a lifetime. The true costs of childhood maltreatment will never be appreciated, and can never be avoided, until clinicians, researchers and policy makers become aware of the core concepts of neurodevelopment and the neuorarcheology of child maltreatment. Acknowledgements This work was supported, in part, by the Brown Family Foundation, the Hogg Foundation for Mental Health, Children's Justice Act/Court Improvement Act, Texas Department of Protective and Regulatory Services, Maconda O'Connor and the Pritzker Cousins Foundation. References Ackerman, P.T., Newton, J.E., McPHerson, W.B., Jones, J.G., & Dykman, R.A. (1998). Prevalence of post traumatic stress disorder and other psychiatric diagnoses in three groups of abused children (sexual, physical, and both). Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 759-774. Alexander, P.C., Anderson, C.L., Brand, B., Schaeffer, C.M., Grelling, B.Z., & Kretz, L. (1998). Adult attachment and longterm effects in survivors of incest. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 45-61. Altman, J., & Das, G.D. (1964). Autoradiographic examination of the effects of enriched environment on the rate of glial multipication in the adult rat brain. Nature, 204, 1161-1165. Bell, D., & Belicki, K. (1998). A community-based study of well-being in adults reporting childhood abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 681-685. Bennett, E.L., Diamond, M.L., Krech, D., & Rosenzweig, M.R. (1964). Chemical and anatomical plasticity of the brain. Science, 146, 610-619. Briggs, L., & Joyce, P.R. (1997). What determines post-traumatic stress disorder symptomatology for survivors of childhood sexual abuse? Child Abuse & Neglect, 21, 575-582. Coleman, P.D., & Riesen, A.H. (1968). Environmental effects on cortical dendritc fields: I. rearing in the dark. Journal of Anatomy (London), 102, 363-374. Cummins, R.A., & Livesey, P. (1979). Enrichment-isolation, cortex length, and the rank order effect. Brain Research, 178, 88-98. Darwin, C. (1868). The variations of animals and plants under domestication. London:J. Murray De Bellis, M.D., Keshavan, M.S., Clark, D.B., Casey, B.J., Giedd, J.N., Boring, A.M., Frustaci, K., & Ryan, N.D. (1999). Developmental traumatology part II: brain development. Biol Psychiat, 45, 1271-1284. Dennis, W. (1973). Children of the Creche. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Diamond, M.C., & Hopson, J. (1998). Magic Trees of the Mind: How to nurture your child's intelligence, creativity, and healthy emotions from birth through adolescence. New York: Dutton. Diamond, M.C., Krech, D., & Rosenzweig, M.R. (1964). The effects of an enriched environment on the histology of the rat cerebral cortex. Comparative Neurology, 123, 111-119. Diamond, M.C., Law, F., Rhodes, H., Lindner, B., Rosenzweig, M.R., Krech, D., & Bennett, E.L. (1966). Increases in cortical depth and glia numbers in rats subjected to enriched environments. Comparative Neurology, 128, 117-126. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Fourth Edition (DSM IV). (1994). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Ebinger, P. (1974). A cytoachitectonic volumetric comparison of brains in wild and domestic sheep. Z.Anat.Entwicklungsgesch, 144, 267-302. Epstein, J.N., Saunders, B.E., Kilpatrick, D.G., & Resnick, H.S. (1998). PTSD as a mediator between childhood rape and alcohol use in adult women. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 223-234. Felliti, V.J., Anda, R.F., Nordenberg, D., Wiallamson, D.F., Spitz, A.M., Edwards, V., Koss, M.P., & Marks, J.S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: the Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14, 245-258. Fergusson, D.M., & Horwood, L.J. (1998). Exposure to interparental violence in childhood and psychological adjustment in young adulthood. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 339-357. Ford, J.D., & Kidd, P. (1998). Early childhood trauma and disorders of extreme stress and predictors of treatment outcome with chronic posttramatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 11, 743-761. Friedrich, W.N. (1998). Behavioral manifestations of child sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 523-531. Gould, E., Reeves, A.J., Graziano, M.S.A., & Gross, C.G. (1999). Neurogenesis in the neocortex of adult primates. Science, 286, 548-552. Greenough, W.T., & Volkmar, F.R. (1973). Pattern of dendritic branching in occipital cortex of rats reared in complex environments. Experimential Neurology, 40, 491-504. Greenough, W.T., Volkmar, F.R., & Juraska, J.M. (1973). Effects of rearing complexity on dendritic branching in frontolateral and temporal cortex of the rat. Experimental Neurology, 41, 371-378. Haddad, P., & Garralda, M. (1992). Hyperkinetic syndrome and disruptive early experiences . British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 700-703. Hadi, F.A., & Llabre, M.M. (1998). The Gulf crisis experience of Kuwaiti children: Psychological and cognitive factors. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 11, 45-56. Heidenreich, F.W. (1834). Kaspar Hausers verwundung, krankeit und liechenoffnung. Journal der Chirurgie und Augen-Heilkunde, 21 (1834), 91-123. Hertzman, C., & Wiens, M. (1996). Child development and long-term outcomes: a population health perspective and summary of successful interventions. Soc.Sci.Med., 43, 1083-1095. Hubel, D.H., & Wiesel, T.N. (1963). Receptive fields of cells in striate cortex of very young, visually inexperienced kittens. Journal of Neurophysiology, 26, 994-1002. Hubel, D.H., & Wiesel, T.N. (1970). The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. Journal of Physiology, 206, 419-436. Huttenlocher, P.R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex: developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163, 195-205. Huttenlocher, P.R. (1994). Synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. In G. Dawson & K.W. Fischer (Eds.), Human Behavior and the Developing Brain. (pp. 35-54). New York: Guilford. Kaufman, J. (1991). Depressive disorders in maltreated children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30 (2), 257-265. Kilpatrick, K.L., & Williams, L.M. (1998). Potential mediators of post-traumatic stress disorder in child witnesses to domestic violence. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 319-330. Kuan, C.-Y., Roth, K.A., Flavell, R.A., & Rakic, P. (2000). Mechanisms of programmed cell death in the developing brain . Trends in Neuroscience, 23, 291-297. Lauder, J.M. (1988). Neurotransmitters as morphogens. Progress in Brain Research, 73, 365-388. Meaney, M.J., Aitken, D.H., van Berkal, C., Bhatnagar, S., & Sapolsky, R.M. (1988). Effect of neonatal handling on age-related impairments associated with the hippocampus. Science, 239 :766-768. Molnar, B.E., Shade, S.B., Kral, A.H., Booth, R.E., & Watters, J.K. (1998). Suicidal behavior and sexual/physical abuse among street youth. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 213-222. Money, J., & Annecillo, C. (1976). IQ changes following change of domicile in the syndrome of reversible hyposomatotropinism (psychosocial dwarfism): pilot investigation . Psychoneuroendocrinology, 1, 427-429. Mulder, R.T., Fergusson, D.M., Beautrais, A.L., & Joyce, P.R. (1998). Relationship between dissociation, childhood sexual abuse, childhood physical abuse, and mental illness in a general population sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 806-811. O'Connor, C., Rutter, M., & English and Romanian Adoptees study team. (2000). Attachment disorder behavior following early severe deprivation: extension and longitudinal follow-up. J.Am.Acad.Child Adolesc.Psychiatry, 39, 703-712. O'Keefe, M. (1995). Predictors of child abuse in maritally violent families. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10, 3-25. Ornitz, E.M., & Pynoos, R.S. (1989). Startle modulation in children with post-traumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 866-870. Orr, S.P., Lasko, N.B., Metzger, L.J., Berry, N.J., Ahern, C.E., & Pitman, R.K. (1998). Psychophysiologic assessment of women with posttraumatic stress disorder resulting from childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 906-913. Pelcovitz, D., Kaplan, S., Goldenberg, B.A., Mandel, F., Lehane, J., & Guarrera, J. (1994). Post-traumatic stress disorder in physically abused adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33: (305). 312 Perry, B.D. (1988). Placental and blood element neurotransmitter receptor regulation in humans: potential models for studying neurochemical mechanisms underlying behavioral teratology. Progress in Brain Research, 73, 189-206. Perry, B.D. (1994). Neurobiological sequelae of childhood trauma: post-traumatic stress disorders in children. In M. Murberg (Ed.), Catecholamines in Post-traumatic Stress Disorder: Emerging Concepts. (pp. 253-276). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Press. Perry, B.D. (1998). Anxiety Disorders. In C.E. Coffey & R.A. Brumback (Eds.), Textbook of Pediatric Neuropsychiatry. (pp. 579-594). Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Press, Inc. Perry, B.D. (1999). The memories of states: how the brain stores and retrieves traumatic experience. In J.M. Goodwin & R. Attias (Eds.), Splintered Reflections: Images of the Body In Trauma. (pp. 9-38). New York: Basic Books. Perry, B.D. (2000). The neurodevelopmental impact of violence in childhood. In D. Schetky & E. Benedek (Eds.), Textbook of Child and Adolescent Forensic Psychiatry. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Press, Inc. Perry, B.D., & Pollard, R. (1997). Altered brain development following global neglect in early childhood. Proceedings from the Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting (New Orleans), (abstract) Perry, B.D., & Pollard, R. (1998). Homeostasis, stress, trauma, and adaptation: A neurodevelopmental view of childhood trauma. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 7, 33-51. Perry, B.D., Pollard, R.A., Baker, W.L., Sturges, C., Vigilante, D., & Blakley, T.L. (1995). Continuous heartrate monitoring in maltreated children. Annual Meeting of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, New Research, (abstract) Perry, B.D., Pollard, R.A., Blakley, T.L., Baker, W.L., & Vigilante, D. (1995). Childhood trauma, the neurobiology of adaptation and use-dependent development of the brain: How states become traits. Infant Mental Health Journal, 16, 271-291. Pfefferbaum, B. (1997). Posttraumatic stress disorder in children: A review of the past 10 years. J.Am.Acad.Child Adolesc.Psychiatry, 36, 1503-1511. Pfefferbaum, B. (Ed.) (1998). Stress in Children. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Company. Plotsky, P.M., & Meaney, M.J. (1993). Early, postnatal experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin releasing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content and stress-induced release in adult rats. Molec Brain Res, 18, 195-200. Rakic, P. (1981). Development of visual centers in the primate brain depends upon binocular competition before birth. Science, 214, 928-931. Rakic, P. (1996). Development of cerebral cortex in human and non-human primates. In M. Lewis (Ed.), Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: A Comprehensive Textbook. (pp. 9-30). New York: Williams and Wilkins. Rehkamper, G., Haase, E., & Frahm, H.D. (1988). Allometric comparison of brain weight and brain structure volumes in different breeds of the domestic pigeon, columbia livia f. d. Brain Behav.Evol., 31, 141-149. Rhodes, N., & Hutchinson, S. (1994). Labor experiences of childhood sexual abuse survivors. Birth, 21, 213-220. Rohrs, M. (1955). Vergleichende untersuchungen an wild- und hauskatzen. Zool.Anz., 155, 53-69. Rohrs, M., & Ebinger, P. (1978). Die beurteilung von hirngrobenunterschieden zwischen wild- und haustieren. Z.zool.Syst.Evolut.-forsch, 16, 1-14. Rorty, M., & Yager, J. (1996). Histories of childhood trauma and complex post-traumatic sequelae in women with eating disorders. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 19, Ruchkin, V.V., Eisemann, M., & Hagglof, B. (1998). Juvenile male rape victims: Is the level of post-traumatic stress related to personality and parenting. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 889-899. Rutledge, L.T., Wright, C., & Duncan, J. (1974). Morphological changes in pyramidal cells of mammalian neocortex associated with increased use. Experimental Neurology, 44, 209-228. Rutter, M., Andersen-Wood, L., Beckett, C., Bredenkamp, D., Castle, J., Grootheus, C., Keppner, J., Keaveny, L., Lord, C., O'Connor, T.G., & English and Romanian Adoptees study team. (1999). Quasi-autistic patterns following severe early global privation. J.Child Psychol.Psychiat., 40, 537-49. Rutter, M., & English and Romanian Adoptees study team. (1998). Developmental catch-up, and deficit, following adoption after severe global early privation. J.Child Psychol.Psychiat., 39, 465-476. Schaaf, K.K., & McCanne, T.R. (1998). Relationship of childhood sexual, physical and combined sexual and physical abuse to adult victimization and posttraumatic stress disorder. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 1119-1133. Spitz, R.A. (1945). Hospitalism: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early childhood. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 1:53-74. Spitz, R.A. (1946). Hospitalism: A follow-up report on investigation described in Volume I, 1945. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 2:113-117. Strathearn,L.; Gray,P.H.; O'Callaghan,M.J.; Wood,D.W. (submitted) Cognitive neurodevelopment in extremely low birth weight infants: nature vs. nurture revisited Stuber, M.L., Kazak, A.E., Meeske, K., Barakat, L., Guthrie, D., Garnier, H., Pynoos, R., & Meadows, A. (1997). Predictors of posttraumatic stress symptoms in childhood cancer survivors . Pediatrics, 100, 958-964. Terr, L. (1991). Childhood traumas: an outline and overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1-20. Uno, H., Tarara, R., Else, J., & et.al. (1989). Hippocampal damage associated with prolonged and fatal stress in primates . Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 1705-1711. Winje, D., & Ulvik, A. (1998). Long-term outcome of trauma in children: The psychological consequences of a bus accident. J.Child Psychol.Psychiat., 39, 635-642

Labels: , , , , , ,

----- -------- /* ----------------------------------------------- Blogger Template Style Name: Son of Moto (Mean Green Blogging Machine variation) Designer: Jeffrey Zeldman URL: www.zeldman.com Date: 23 Feb 2004 ----------------------------------------------- */ /* Primary layout */ body { margin: 0; padding: 0; border: 0; text-align: center; color: #554; background: #692 url(http://www.blogblog.com/moto_son/outerwrap.gif) top center repeat-y; font: small tahoma, "Bitstream Vera Sans", "Trebuchet MS", "Lucida Grande", lucida, helvetica, sans-serif; } img { border: 0; display: block; } /* Wrapper */ @media all { #wrapper { margin: 0 auto; padding: 0; border: 0; width: 692px; text-align: left; background: #fff url(http://www.blogblog.com/moto_son/innerwrap.gif) top right repeat-y; font-size:90%; } } @media handheld { #wrapper { width: 90%; } } /* Header */ #blog-header { color: #ffe; background: #8b2 url(http://www.blogblog.com/moto_son/headbotborder.gif) bottom left repeat-x; margin: 0 auto; padding: 0 0 15px 0; border: 0; } #blog-header h1 { font-size: 24px; text-align: left; padding: 15px 20px 0 20px; margin: 0; background-image: url(http://www.blogblog.com/moto_son/topper.gif); background-repeat: repeat-x; background-position: top left; } #blog-header p { font-size: 110%; text-align: left; padding: 3px 20px 10px 20px; margin: 0; line-height:140%; } /* Inner layout */ #content { padding: 0 20px; } @media all { #main { width: 400px; float: left; } #sidebar { width: 226px; float: right; } } @media handheld { #main { width: 100%; float: none; } #sidebar { width: 100%; float: none; } } /* Bottom layout */ #footer { clear: left; margin: 0; padding: 0 20px; border: 0; text-align: left; border-top: 1px solid #f9f9f9; background-color: #fdfdfd; } #footer p { text-align: left; margin: 0; padding: 10px 0; font-size: x-small; background-color: transparent; color: #999; } /* Default links */ a:link, a:visited { font-weight : bold; text-decoration : none; color: #692; background: transparent; } a:hover { font-weight : bold; text-decoration : underline; color: #8b2; background: transparent; } a:active { font-weight : bold; text-decoration : none; color: #692; background: transparent; } /* Typography */ #main p, #sidebar p { line-height: 140%; margin-top: 5px; margin-bottom: 1em; } .post-body { line-height: 140%; } h2, h3, h4, h5 { margin: 25px 0 0 0; padding: 0; } h2 { font-size: large; } h3.post-title { margin-top: 5px; font-size: medium; } ul { margin: 0 0 25px 0; } li { line-height: 160%; } #sidebar ul { padding-left: 10px; padding-top: 3px; } #sidebar ul li { list-style: disc url(http://www.blogblog.com/moto_son/diamond.gif) inside; vertical-align: top; padding: 0; margin: 0; } dl.profile-datablock { margin: 3px 0 5px 0; } dl.profile-datablock dd { line-height: 140%; } .profile-img {display:inline;} .profile-img img { float:left; margin:0 10px 5px 0; border:4px solid #8b2; } #comments { border: 0; border-top: 1px dashed #eed; margin: 10px 0 0 0; padding: 0; } #comments h3 { margin-top: 10px; margin-bottom: -10px; font-weight: normal; font-style: italic; text-transform: uppercase; letter-spacing: 1px; } #comments dl dt { font-weight: bold; font-style: italic; margin-top: 35px; padding: 1px 0 0 18px; background: transparent url(http://www.blogblog.com/moto_son/commentbug.gif) top left no-repeat; color: #998; } #comments dl dd { padding: 0; margin: 0; } .deleted-comment { font-style:italic; color:gray; } /* Feeds ----------------------------------------------- */ #blogfeeds { } #postfeeds { }

Adopt Biomed

This blog gathers information about biomedical interventions for children with adoption trauma and Reactive Attachment Disorder. Posts are gathered from multiple websites in one place. Most posts contain unedited text relating to biomedical treatment, dietary changes, vitamins, homeopathy, herbs, etc. Where possible, the link to the original information is included.

Saturday, September 8, 2007

EFAs and behavior

What are EFAs? · Nutrition: Essential Fatty Acids (EFAs): What are EFAs? Things to Avoid · Nutrition: Essential Fatty Acids (EFAs): Things to Avoid Omega-3 Deficiency Symptoms List of Symptoms "These signs include · dry hair, · dry skin (often noticed as a 'goosebump' rash on the upper arms and/or upper thighs), · excessive thirst, · frequent urination, · problems with attention and so on." ILT: Essential Fatty Acids (EFAs) Keratosis Pilaris Keratosis Pilaris: Definition "Keratosis pilaris is a common skin condition that looks like small goose bumps, which are actually dead skin cells that build up around the hair follicle." HealthAtoZ.com: Keratosis Pilaris Keratosis Pilaris: Description · "Keratosis pilaris is a disorder that occurs around the hair follicles of the upper arms, thighs, and sometimes the buttocks. · It presents as small, benign bumps or papules that are actually waxy build-ups of keratin. Normally skin sloughs off. However, around the hair follicle where the papules form, the keratinized skin cells slough off at a slower rate, clogging the follicles. · This is generally thought to be genetic disorder, although the symptoms of keratosis pilaris are often seen with ichthyosis and allergic dermatitis. It can also be observed in people of all ages who have either inherited it or have a vitamin A deficiency or have dry skin. · Keratosis pilaris is a self-limiting disorder that disappears as the person ages. · It can become more severe when conditions are dry such as during the winter months or in dry climates." HealthAtoZ.com: Keratosis Pilaris Keratosis Pilaris: Causes and Symptoms "The specific causes of this disorder are unknown. Since this disorder runs in families, it is thought to be hereditary. Keratosis pilaris is not a serious disorder and is not contagious. The symptoms of keratosis pilaris are based on the development of small white papules the size of a grain of sand on the upper arms, thighs, and occasionally the buttocks and face. The papules occur around a hair follicle and are firm and white. They feel a little like coarse sandpaper, but they are not painful and there usually is no itching associated with them. They are easily removed and the material inside the papule usually contains a small, coiled hair." HealthAtoZ.com: Keratosis Pilaris Keratosis Pilaris: Treatment · "To treat keratosis pilaris patients can try several strategies to lessen the bumps. · First, the patient can supplement the natural removal of dry skin and papules by using a loofah or another type of scrub showering or bathing. · A variety of different over-the-counter (OTC) lotions, ointments, and creams can also be applied after showering while the skin is still moist and then several times a day to keep the area moist. · Medicated lotions with urea, 15% alphahydroxy acids, or Retin A can also be prescribed by the dermatologist and applied one to two times daily. · Systemic (oral) medications are not prescribed for keratosis pilaris. · However if papules are opened and become infected, antibiotics may be necessary to treat the infection." HealthAtoZ.com: Keratosis Pilaris "I did a little research on vitamin a deficiency and it seems to be, if not a cause, then a trigger to a lot of skin problems. However, taking vitamin A does not necessarily eliminate the deficiency. I know this because I was taking a vitamin A/B combo and nothing was happening. Then after doing some reading about eczema…omega 3 fatty acids (found in cod liver oil supplements [caution: cod liver oil in the quantity necessary to get three grams per day of omega-3's would contain dangerous levels of vitamin A]) help the body absorb vitamin A. This has worked for me and has offset a lot of the uncomfortable dry skin associated with not only my eczema, but also on my arms and legs (KP)." KeratosisPilaris.org: Archives: Re: Vitamin A and KP · Essential Fatty Acids (EFAs): Things to Avoid: Too Much Fish, Farm-Raised Fish · Essential Fatty Acids (EFAs): Things to Avoid: Dangerous Levels of Vitamin A · Nutrition: Vitamins and Minerals: Liquid Multivitamins: Absorption · Nutrition: Vitamins and Minerals: Vitamin A: Absorption Effects ADD, Dyslexia "If a child does not get enough EFAs, the myelin sheath protecting the axons of billions of neurons may not be adequate. Recent research is showing that boys with ADD have EFA deficiencies (Stevens, L. et al, 1995:000-000) and similar deficiencies are possibly responsible for the symptoms of dyslexia. (Stordy & Nicholl, 2000:105)." ILT: Essential Fatty Acids (EFAs) Depression "When researchers monitor eating habits across countries, they find that as fish consumption goes up, depression rates go down. In fact, there is a sixty-fold difference in depression rates across countries from the highest (Japan and Taiwan) to the lowest (North America, Europe and New Zealand) omega-3 fat consumption. Even postpartum depression decreases as women increase their consumption of fish. Many people also report a drop in mood when they switch too quickly to a low-fat diet. (Page 156)" Book: Somer, Elizabeth, M.A., R.D. Food & Mood. Henry Holt and Company, LLC, 1999. Serotonin and Dopamine "Over the last decade, neuroscientists have been examining the consequences of omega-3 deficiencies in the central nervous system. Alterations in serotonin and dopamine levels, as well as the functioning of these two important neurotransmitters is evident in an omega-3 deficiency. The changes observed in omega-3 deficiency in animals is strikingly similar to that found in autopsy studies of human depression.20" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Blood-Brain Barrier "In addition to changing serotonin and dopamine levels and functioning, omega-3 deficiencies are known to compromise the blood-brain barrier, which normally protects the brain from unwanted matter gaining access.21" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Blood Flow "Omega-3 deficiency can also decrease normal blood flow to the brain,22,23 an interesting finding given the studies which show that patients with depression have compromised blood flow to a number of brain regions.24,25" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Antidepressant Activity "Finally, omega-3 deficiency also causes a 35 percent reduction in brain phosphatidylserine (PS) levels.26 This is also of relevance when considering that PS has documented antidepressant activity in humans.27,28" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Mechanisms of EPA/DHA Regulation of Mood "DHA [an omega-3 fatty acid] is found in high levels in the cells of the central nervous system (neurons); here it acts as a form of scaffolding for structural support.29 When omega-3 intake is inadequate, the nerve cell becomes stiff as cholesterol and omega-6 fatty acids are substituted for omega-3.30 When a nerve cell becomes rigid, proper neurotransmission from cell to cell and within cells will be compromised.31 While DHA provides structure and helps to ensure normal neurotransmission, EPA [an omega-3 fatty acid] may be more important in the signaling within nerve cells.32 Normalizing communications within nerve cells has been suggested to be an important factor in alleviating depressive symptoms.33" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH "In addition, EPA can lower the levels of two important immune chemicals, · tumour necrosis factor alpha (TNFa) and · interleukin 1 beta (IL-1ß), · as well as prostaglandin E2.34 All three of these chemicals are elevated in depression.35-38 In fact, higher levels of TNFa and IL-1ß are associated with severity of depression.39" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH "Finally, EPA has been hypothesized to increase brain-derived neurotropic factor (BDNF), which is known to be lower in depressed patients.20 BDNF is neuroprotective, enhances neurotransmission, has antidepressant activity and supports normal brain structure. BDNF may prevent the death of nerve cells in depression." New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Correcting an Omega-3 Deficiency Note: More information about sources of omega-3s can be found on the page Nutrition: Essential Fatty Acid (EFA) Sources Flax "Flax, our richest source of omega 3, quickly replenishes a long-standing omega 3 deficiency. A dozen 8 oz. bottles of good quality flax oil consumed over the course of a few months will suffice [approximately one bottle every five days, 1.6 ounces per day]." The Edelson Center: Essential Fatty Acids: The Healing Fats "There have been some published case reports indicating that flaxseed oil may be helpful in cases of bipolar depression and the anxiety disorder agoraphobia.40 The first controlled clinical trial indicating that omega-3 fatty acids may be of benefit in depression was published in 1999. In this case, 9:6 g ['9:6 g' should be read a dose of 6 g per day for a period of nine days?] of EPA/DHA versus placebo led to longer periods of remission and improvement in depressive symptoms in those with bipolar depression.41" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH 2g Daily of Pure EPA "Some researchers theorize that such high doses of EPA/DHA may not be necessary and that low levels of pure EPA may be of benefit.32 In a study published in the American Journal of Psychiatry, researchers showed that just 2g of pure EPA could improve the symptoms of treatment-resistant depression. The researchers found that the EPA (versus placebo), when added to an ineffective antidepressant for one month, significantly improved depressive symptoms.42" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH 1g Daily of Pure EPA "A larger study published in Archives of General Psychiatry replicated these findings, however, this time various doses of EPA were examined. Those on ineffective antidepressants were given 1g, 2g or 4g of pure EPA or a placebo in addition to the medication. Interestingly, the 1g daily dose of EPA led to the most significant improvements over the three-month study; it appeared that less was more. There were significant improvements in depressive symptoms, sleep, anxiety, lassitude, libido and thoughts of suicide.43" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH "Harvard researchers have also shown that just 1g of pure EPA is beneficial in the treatment of borderline personality disorder. This personality disorder, which is particularly difficult to treat, is characterized by both depressive and aggressive symptoms. This was a two-month placebo-controlled study and the results showed that EPA has a mood-regulating effect, improving both depression and aggression versus placebo.46" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH 4.4g EPA and 2.2g DHA Mix "Researchers from Taiwan Medical University published a recent study in which they found that a 4.4g EPA and 2.2g DHA mix could alleviate depression versus placebo in those with treatment-resistant depression. This was a two-month study involving patients who were on antidepressants that were not working. As with the other omega-3 studies discussed, the fish oil was well tolerated and no adverse events were reported.44" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Antarctic Krill Oil (400mg EPA, 240mg DHA) "There is also evidence that omega-3 oils may be of benefit in treating depressive symptoms outside of major depressive disorder. Canadian researchers showed that Antarctic krill oil (400mg EPA, 240mg DHA) could improve depressive symptoms associated with premenstrual syndrome.45" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH DHA Alone not Recommended "To date, with one exception, all studies conducted on omega-3 fatty acids and mood have had a positive outcome. The singular negative study examined pure DHA in patients with depression. The results in the case showed that DHA alone was no better than placebo in alleviating depressive symptoms.47" New Findings About Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Depression by Alan C. Logan, ND, FRSH Omega-6 Deficiency "Long--term exclusion or excessive use of flax oil can result in omega 6 deficiency after about two years, because flax oil is omega 3 rich but omega 6 poor." The Edelson Center: Essential Fatty Acids: The Healing Fats

Labels: , , , , ,

Wednesday, August 22, 2007

Low Dopamine in Neglected/Abused kids?

Gerra G, Leonardi C, Cortese E, Zaimovic A, Dell'agnello G, Manfredini M, Somaini L, Petracca F, Caretti V, Saracino MA, Raggi MA, Donnini C Homovanillic acid (HVA) plasma levels inversely correlate with attention deficit-hyperactivity and childhood neglect measures in addicted patients. J Neural Transm. 2007 Aug 10; Background. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) seems to be a risk condition for substance use disorders, possibly in relationship to common neurobiological changes, underlying both addictive and externalising behaviour susceptibility. Although this vulnerability has been primarily attributed to gene variants, previous studies suggest that also adverse childhood experiences may influence neurotransmission, affecting in particular brain dopamine (DA) system and possibly concurring to the development of behavioural disorders. Therefore, we decided to investigate ADHD symptoms and plasma concentrations of the DA metabolite homovanillic acid (HVA) in abstinent addicted patients, in comparison with healthy control subjects, evaluating whether ADHD scores were related with HVA levels, as expression of DA turnover, and whether HVA values, in turn, were associated with childhood emotional neglect. Methods. Eighty-two abstinent drug dependent patients, and 44 normal controls, matched for age and sex, completed the Wender Utah Rating Scale (WURS), measuring ADHD symptoms, and the Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse Questionnaire (CECA-Q). Blood samples were collected to determine HVA plasma levels. Results. Addicted individuals showed significantly higher ADHD scores and lower HVA levels respect to control subjects. ADHD scores at WURS in addicted patients negatively correlated with plasma HVA values. In turn, plasma HVA levels were inversely associated with childhood neglect measures, reaching statistical significance with "mother-antipathy" and "mother neglect" scores. Conclusions. These findings suggest the possibility that childhood experience of neglect and poor mother-child attachment may have an effect on central dopamine function as an adult, in turn contributing to both ADHD and substance abuse neurobiological vulnerability.

Labels: , , ,

Wednesday, July 18, 2007

Cofactors for neurotransmitters

Tourette Syndrome, B6 dependency, allergy BonnieGr bonniegr at aol.com Thu Feb 8 13:19:55 EST 1996 I have been putting the puzzle pieces together on the subject of Tourette Syndrome, using medline documents, college textbooks, etc. Please read the following long rationale document that I have prepared, and comment by e-mail (BonnieGr at aol.com). It is my hope that more research will be done to validate my theory. Enjoy! The following addresses TS/OCD/ADD. The diagnosis would lie in the degree of vitamin B6 dependency/deficiency, and how long the person has been in this state. Carl Hansen, Jr. M.D. of Minneapolis describes celiac disease in several of his TS patients. This could be a pathway to vitamin B6 deficiency via malabsorption. Streptococcal infections have also been associated with TS. This could be a combination of the hyaluronidase's (an enzyme produced by the hemolytic strep that depolymerizes the ground substance of tissue) or streptokinase's actions on the blood brain barrier, the drain of vitamin B6 from the bacteria's own useage, the body's requirement of B6 for immunity, and the antibiotic's B6 antagonistic properties. A pre-exising B6 dependency/deficiency could be uncovered. TOURETTE SYNDROME, ALLERGY AND THE B6 DEPENDENCY STATE I have my Bachelor's degree in Biology, specializing in Medical Technology, and in graduate school, I took graduate courses in biochemistry. I work as a medical technologist performing and verifying clinical laboratory tests in Chemistry, Hematology and Blood Bank at Mt.Carmel East Medical Center in Columbus, Ohio. I do not have TS but my son, Jason (13 yrs old) has TS with OCD. ADD has not been formally diagnosed, although he has problems with organization, distractibility, and the ability to switch gears. My son has had allergies since he was a baby. He is sensitive to red dye #40 with tired splitting headaches which make him scream until he is exhausted and sleeps. This, of course, hasn't happened in several years since he has avoided the dye. He also is allergic to sulfa, molds, dust, grass, trees, and most airborn allergens. He has been on the vitamins below for 1.5 months and the teachers have said that he is a different kid. Medications that he had been on made him progressively worse, and so we made a personal decision to discontinue meds altogether. He now is motivated in school, concentrates and finishes his work, and is less disruptive with his tics in class. At home he still has his tics and compulsions, but they are shorter lived and occur less often. He has had a set back this week due to a new semester with a new schedule, plus a very moldy, rainy few days. We gave him a little extra calcium-magnesium and one extra vitamin B3. He said that this gave him relief from his symptoms (he has never said this before with anything else). I solidified my theory on the premise that Jason is probably mildly vitamin B6 dependent. He was either born requiring high amounts of B6, and/or B6 antagonists attacked early in his first year of life. B6 antagonists are hydrazines (plant growth regulators, tartrazine, etc), DOPA found in certain beans, penicillinamine, antioxidants in petroleum, many drugs including penicillin, erythromycin, phenobarbital, tetracycline, corticosteroids, sulfamethoxazole, etc. Amino acids began building up in his system, from decreased transamination, etc. Serotonin became decreased from tryptophan not being able to be utilized. Allergies developed (which is in association with low B6), I believe allergy produces swings in histamine levels which causes a constant fluctuation in neurotransmitters capable of producing mood swings and rages. The conservation of vitamin B6 (when not abundantly available) causes it to be used by the prevailing neurotransmitter system at any given time, leaving other neurotransmitter systems less than optimally functional. Histamine receptors have been found to trigger dopamine receptors directly. Histamine is also a neurotransmitter affected by deficient vitamin B6. Its receptor sites are probably increased to compensate. Kinins released into the body's tissues in response to immune complexes can damage the blood brain barrier, thus altering the sensitivity of brain cells to acetylcholine, serotonin, dopamine, histamine, epinephrine and norepinephrine. I found that L-dopa doesn't readily form dopamine in B6 deficiency, so probably dopamine is reduced causing an increase in dopamine receptor sites along with an increase the norepinephrine and epinephrine (which are formed from dopamine) receptors sites. These increased receptor sites make the nerves more excitable and false transmitters or true neurotransmitters can set them off with explosive qualities. These false transmitters can be phenolic substances, such as food additives, drugs, etc. The enzyme, phenol sulfotransferase (PST), detoxifies and eliminates phenolics (drugs, food additives, serotonin, dopamine (to name a few). In the brain, sulfation is used while glucuronidation prevails elsewhere. Cysteine requires B6 to enzymatically release sulfur for sulfation of these phenols by PST. Considering this, the neurotransmitters would would be conserved to a certain extent (their sulfation and elimination would be slowed down). ADD may happen when these false transmitters create background "noise", and if there is a real message to get through via other neurons, it is masked. When a true message is fired, it may have too strong of a signal, creating a strong impulsion, which can lead to the development of a tic or compulsion if the impulsion is acted upon and repeated creating a sort of conditioned reflex network of nerves. Mental, motor, and vocal tics can develop this way. According to my_ Biochemistry_ by Lehninger textbook from my graduate student in Biology days, tryptophan is broken down in Vitamin B3 deficiency to make nicotinic acid. Tryptophan is found in meat and is plentiful, if you are a meat eater. Tryptophan is the precursor for serotonin. I also looked up Vitamin B3 and how it could be connected to the issues of allergy and serotonin defiency in the brain. I found that Vitamin B3 is used to make NAD, NADP, which are coenzymes used in making histamine and serotonin (to name a few), and are essential in oxidative-reductive cellular metabolism. The B3 is needed due to tryptophan's inability to be broken down to nicotinic acid without adequate B6. So, if Vitamins B3 and B6 are being used for histamine production, then serotonin production suffers. Tryptophan then must be used in a higher frequency to make nicotinic acid. In Vitamin B6 deficiency, this cannot happen, because the enzyme kynurinase, that catalyzes the cleavage of 3 hydroxykynurine (an intermediate in tryptophan catabolism), contains pyridoxal phosphate (an active coenzyme form of Vitamin B6). In Vitamin B6 deficiency, large amounts of L-kynurenine are excreted in the urine, because of its high plasma levels. This is described in "Elevated plasma kynurenine in Tourette syndrome", _Molecular & Chemical Neuropathology_21(1): 55-60,1994 Jan. Kynurenine itself is metabolised to other substances, several of which are known to have effects on neurones. (per a research study done at University College London Medical School Harlow, England by Sheila L. Handley, BPharm, Ph.D. 1994) Large amounts of tryptophan which is broken down to ineffectively try to produce nicotinic acid reduces the amount of serotonin produced. Ineffective tryptophan utilization also uses alot of oxygen with tryptophan 2,3-dioxygenase. Low serotonin levels could cause obsessive compulsive behaviour, depression, and other mood related disorders. B6 is also required for the decarboxylase step of serotonin, histamine, and catecholamine pathways in the brain. In low B6, conservation takes place, so that B6 is used for fewer enzymes. When allergy strikes, the production of histamine causes a further imbalance of neurotransmitters, causing serotonin and/or catecholamine production to be further depleted. Sherry A Rogers, M.D., a specialist in environmental medicine, reports that all of the TS cases she has seen have a least one nutrient deficiency, and usually several. And she notes that all of these patients have hidden mold, dust, chemical and food sensitivities. ("Tourette Syndrome", _Health Counselor_, Vol.7, No.4) Acetylcholine is produced by acetyl CoA and choline. The choline is supplied through lecithin in Jason's supplements. In vitamin B6 deficiency, acetyl CoA would be made by fatty acid oxidation. So acetycholine could be functional with an adequate supply of fatty acids (evening primrose oil or flax oil might be useful). Acetylcholine could be in shorter supply in the parasympathetic system (relaxation) due to overuse in the sympathetic system where norepinephrine usually rules. The parasympathetic nervous system would need to have more acetylcholine in TS and associated disorders, it seems. Relaxation through the parasympathetic nervous system (which uses acetylcholine), where the heart rate is slowed, the blood pressure is lowered, the food is digested well, etc. is difficult in TS. Acetylcholine is probably overactive in the sympathetic autonomic nervous system, trying to stimulate the low supply of catecholamines, which would be decreased due to B6 deficiency/dependency. The receptors sites for catecholamines would be hyperexcitable and increased in number. The net usage of catecholamines could be normal to decreased due to increased stimulation by acetylcholine, depending on the availability of B6 in the body, and the conservation by low sulfation by PST. Conditions of emotional stress are known to produce more ticcing in TS. In short term stress, norepineprine, dopamine, and epineprine should be able to be produced by the conservation tactics of the body, but in long term stress, these would be exhausted, especially when another B6 dependent system is triggered. Likewise, the same would happen when histamine and serotonin are produced in short term and long term allergy. But as you might expect, the short term conditions would be explosive events with all of those increased receptor sites! Acetylcholine is also involved in the contraction of voluntary muscle cells and many other motor nerves, which are in heavy use in TS. Many people with TS are helped by exercise, where cardiac output and increased body temperature over a period of time inhibit the sympathetic nervous system. It may also help to clear toxic waste, such as kynurenine. Adequate water intake would be required to catabolize acetylcholine by cholinesterase. In my opinion acetylcholine is needed in B6 deficiency/dependency to run the nervous system. Fatty acids are essential to its success in this situation. Fatty acids require NADPH2, and NADH2 for their synthesis, and thus Vitamin B3. Water is also an utmost requirement in keeping acetylcholine from becoming a continuous firecracker. Jason has a water bottle close by most times and drinks tons of water. Water has always calmed him down. It may also dilute the kynurenine, excess amino acids and promote their excretion. If you look at the material written on the Canadian Mennonite families that have been studied with Tourette's disorder, you will see a high frequency of autoimmune and rare conditions. These findings are consistent with what one can expect with other Tourette's patients. For example, there is a high frequency of allergic conditions. My informal survey of TS and allergy results from the online TS support group are: With a total of 25 respondents with TS: 96% have allergies (24 out of 25) 56% have mold allergies 72% have obsessive complulsive traits (18 out of 25) 67% of those with obsessive compulsive traits have mold allergies 3 respondents thought they may have mold allergies, but weren't sure 52% have pollen allergies (ragweed, grass, tree, etc) 56% of those with obsessive compulsive traits have pollen allergy 48 % have animal allergies (cats, dogs, horse) 39% of those with obsessive compulsive traits have animal allergies 40% have dust allergy 39% of those with obsessive compulsive traits have dust allergy 20% have penicillin allergy 28 % of those with obsessive compulsive traits have penicillin allergy 20 % have miscellaneous allergies 11% of those with obsessive compulsive traits have miscellaneous allergies 16 % have food allergies 22 % of those with obsessive complulsive traits have food allergies 8% have sulfa allergy 11% of those with obsessive compulsive traits have sulfa allergy All of our frequent posters responded. The types of allergies are typically respiratory and airborne. Molds and pollens are the top allergens. 79% of the people with mold allergies also had pollen allergies, which are seasonal. Bonnie Grimaldi, BSMT (ASCP) 11283 Meadowcroft St. Pickerington, Ohio 43147 (614) 837-7545

Labels: , , , , ,

Monday, July 2, 2007

Neuroscience -- biomed for neurotransmitters

looksmart FindArticles > Townsend Letter for Doctors and Patients > Jan, 2007 > Article > Print friendly Neurotransmitter Balancing, implemented properly: an indispensable clinical tool Pam Machemehl Helmly Introduction Identifying and treating neurotransmitter (NT) deficiencies and imbalances represent a vast new frontier in upgrading health care. It's only in the last five years that easy-to-use, accurate, and noninvasive tests for neurotransmitter deficiencies have become available. Yet, such tests are under-utilized, and many practitioners remain unfamiliar with NT treatment. Even though it's relatively new, a number of studies in this burgeoning field confirm its powerful effects. (1-2) [ILLUSTRATION OMITTED] A recent study of 78 Russian orphans (adopted into the US), conducted by Dr. Karyn Purvis at Texas Christian University, demonstrated treatment efficacy. The children had severe behavioral disorders, triggered in part by NT imbalances, which were identified through urine testing and addressed through supplementation. In the study, the children were divided into two groups. Immediately after initial testing, the treatment group received neurotransmitter support for two months. The control group was tested, but then waited two months before receiving the supplements. After two months, the treatment group showed significant improvements in both their neurotransmitter test results and in certain tested behaviors including anxiety/depression, thought process, attention problems, and aggressive behavior. "These improvements suggest that amino acid therapy has promise as an intervention for behaviorally disordered children," the researchers concluded. The study findings have been published in the Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine. Dr. Purvis' research (3) confirms what I've observed in my own work with over 4,200 clients in the last eight years: a significant part of a patient's emotional health is determined by his (or her) brain chemistry. Achieving psychological balance is just one of the countless health benefits attainable through addressing brain chemistry. NT imbalances cause (and contribute to) an astonishingly wide range of ailments and symptoms, including depression, anxiety, ADD/ADHD, fatigue, lack of drive, restlessness, anxiety, sleep cycle disorders, migraine headaches, palpitations, immune system suppression, hormonal imbalance, focus issues, memory loss, poor coordination and motor skills, weight gain, food cravings, eating disorders, weight loss, addictions, and chronic pain. Many of these symptoms undermine a patient's ability to comply with health care regimens, making it crucial to address underlying NT issues early on. Yet many clinicians lack the know-how to do so successfully. As one of the few clinicians with a full-time practice devoted exclusively to this specialty, I've been active in this field since its infancy. I founded my Texas-based online company, Neurogistics, to meet the needs of both clinicians and patients. For testing, we rely on labs that utilize the authentic German assays supported by normative data to ensure accurate results and optimum ranges. Moreover, all protocols we generate are based on an extensive logic I developed and individually fine-tune for each patient to insure that nothing is missed, and our scientific team reviews every protocol. Neurotransmitter Depletions in Brief Neurotransmitters are brain chemicals that communicate information throughout the brain and body, relaying signals between neurons. Functioning in dynamic balance are two kinds of neurotransmitters: the excitatory (such as norepinephrine), which stimulate, and the inhibitory (such as serotonin), which calm the brain to balance mood. When the excitatory neurotransmitters are overactive, the inhibitory become depleted. By signaling the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems, NTs act to regulate cardiac function, breathing, and digestion, along with mood, sleep, concentration, and even weight. When out-of-balance, they can trigger many diverse symptoms, and that's why treating them offers the single most significant improvement in overall patient care. Scientists have estimated that as many as 86% of Americans may experience suboptimal neurotransmitter levels. Stress, poor diet, neurotoxins, genetic predisposition, age, drug use (prescription and recreational), and alcohol and caffeine intake can significantly deplete neurotransmitters levels. Defining a New Approach When it comes to balancing brain chemistry, treatment must be based on an accurate analysis rather than guesswork. For example, some practitioners routinely address weight loss and carbohydrate addiction with glutamine supplementation. But without knowing an individual's precise glutamate levels, this can be risky. Glutamine can convert to glutamate, and exacerbate symptoms such as anxiety, restlessness, and sleep cycle issues. Without NT testing and a comprehensive approach, it's easy to make mistakes. With over 200 plus neurotransmitters, individualexacerbations to the 200th power can occur. Neither the symptom picture nor the individual numbers alone can tell you exactly how to restore balance. What's more, the relationship and ratios between the neurotransmitter levels is key--not just the levels themselves. Assessing and treating the overall picture is not "plug and play." Successful NT treatment has four cornerstones: 1. Proper urine testing with values based on accurate normative data 2. Symptom picture and patient history 3. Customized protocols based on the two cornerstones above and guided by clinical expertise 4. Targeted supplementation Evaluation and treatments based on only one cornerstone would at best be incomplete or imprecise and, at worst, potentially harmful, as can be seen from the following case: Case # 1 At age 86, Tom, a retired engineer, had sleep disturbances, waking every couple of hours throughout the night. Though impressively independent and highly functional for a man his age, he also reported that his memory was slipping a bit. Had I relied solely upon a checklist to determine Tom's protocol, I could have easily been mislead by his symptoms and might have treated the wrong neurotransmitters or under-dosed him. His test results revealed that extremely low levels of norepinephrine (and serotonin) were causing his sleeplessness. If I used a checklist without these test results, I might have treated serotonin and dopamine only--which would have delayed improvement. As a result, he might well have discontinued treatment. Instead, with the lab results pointing to low norepinephrine as the causative factor, I first treated his serotonin levels and then, later, added excitatory support. Soon after, his sleep began to improve. Today, Tom sleeps through the night without waking and reports that his memory has also improved dramatically. Mastering the art of accurately synthesizing and interpreting NT results takes years, and there's no substitute for clinical experience. While some companies and laboratories offer a dozen or more standing protocols for use with certain ranges of test results, in my opinion, these don't accurately capture that complexity. To date, lab personnel do not have any clinical experience to draw from when they create protocols. That's why clinicians should not fully rely upon lab report protocols, although in the absence of proper training, many do. The Neurogistics Brain Wellness Program was created as a service to clinicians to fill this gap. Testing I'm well able to evaluate the merits of different testing options, since over an eight-year period, I've seen over 8,500 results from varied testing modalities. In my view, the 24-hour urine testing was of limited use. When NT testing and evaluation were first developed by some of the luminaries in Neurotransmitter Science, I was fortunate to enter this new field, and work and learn from these pioneers. Urinary neurotransmitter testing provided the first non-invasive and accurate guide to brain chemistry. In a recent Townsend Letter article (October 2006), Julia Ross critiques urine testing as inaccurate. It's certainly true that it took a few years to determine sound reference ranges, but given the rapid evolution in this field, it's a mistake to dismiss the current state-of-the-art testing based on poor results of the past. In recent years, urinary testing has provided a reliable basis for treatment. Today, there are over 500,000 cases collected by three labs that document the validity of both the testing method and corresponding treatment. In her article, Ross also claims that a symptom checklist alone is more reliable, but I strongly disagree. Prior to the advent of urine testing, like the early pioneers in this field, I utilized a checklist for symptoms. But no matter how good the checklist, it wasn't sufficiently accurate to alleviate my patients' symptoms. Although, today, I use a questionnaire to help screen for appropriate tests, as well as to monitor treatment results, there's no point in "guessing" which neurotransmitters require balancing when you can know with scientific certainty. Moreover, an incomplete picture can lead to inaccurate treatment, as in the following case: Case #2 Claire, a young professional in her mid-thirties presented with high anxiety and carbohydrate cravings. Based on a checklist alone, I might have assumed I should give her GABA and perhaps glutamine. Yet the test results revealed a serotonin deficiency, while the patient's GABA level was fine. Why risk creating more glutamate (as can occur with glutamine supplementation) when serotonin was the major culprit? Elevated glutamate can increase anxiety, and in this case, Claire would not have experienced long-term resolution from treating GABA. However, Claire enjoyed full resolution of symptoms with serotonin support. Laboratory results reveal the precise levels and ratios of the range of neurotransmitters. Those can't be derived or quantified from a checklist. In this, neurotransmitter treatment is just like other testing procedures. I would never use a questionnaire to identify a pathogenic bacteria, parasite, or Candida. Instead, I would run a comprehensive diagnostic stool analysis. I would feel remiss in my duty if I did not use the best analytical tools available at any given time, and currently, for NT, the best non-invasive method is urinalysis. Added Benefits of Testing In my opinion, it's not enough to merely identify a deficiency. Test results allow you to quantify a given excretion level. Without that number, you might sub-therapeutically dose a patient and fail to alleviate the symptoms. Conversely, giving too high a dosage can also produce problems. For example, Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) teens with drug and/or alcohol problems may have severe serotonin deficiencies. While it's sometimes advisable to put them on a high dosage to bring back their levels, it's important to retest to assure that the levels don't return too quickly. If that were to happen, they could very well flood serotonin receptor sites and experience the same depression and anxiety with which they initially presented. Testing allows you to appropriately adjust treatment. Quantifying levels also indicates when to expect improvement so that patients persist with treatment until they get results. My many Type A clients (such as professional athletes) undertake NT treatment to optimize their brains and their response to stress. Companies rely on our services to improve overall job performance. What's more, testing can reveal incipient problems. I often see executives at the top of their game, just humming, with test results revealing serotonin at a low of 42. If they later experience a major stress, they may find it hard to recuperate. Detecting the problem and treating it early present a significant preventative. With women patients, I often find that after optimizing brain function, they can lower their hormone supplementation to physiological dosages (about 18 mg in the case of progesterone, for example), rather than having to utilize the higher prescription dosages. Why overload yourself with hormones (whether bio-identical or equine), which the body must metabolize to excrete and carry a cancer risk, when through NT balancing, you can use a physiologic dose for its hormonal benefits? Finally, appropriate supplementation is the fifth cornerstone of successful treatment. The supplements we offer target imbalances, while performing the following: * omitting co-factors that interact unfavorably with other practitioner-recommended supplements, * eliminating ingredients potentially problematic for certain health issues (like autism), * avoiding improper combinations of inhibitory and excitatories offered too early in treatment, and * providing ingredient levels appropriate for certain ages or conditions. Balancing Neurotransmitters for Family Health Treating a whole family for neurotransmitter imbalances can improve the relationships and quality of family life. It's fascinating to me how frequently testing reveals that family members share similar neurotransmitter imbalances, although individual symptoms and treatment needs differ. Case #3 When all three members of the Lester family came to me, they were crying out for help, "We are a high-intensity, stressed-out family," Mike, the father, (aged 45) bluntly told me. He had a low libido, while Barbara, the mother, (aged 43) was significantly overweight. Gabe, their son, (aged 13) was defiant, irritable, and unfocused, with behavioral problems experienced at home and in school. Family members were argumentative and irritable with frequent high-decibel explosions. Testing revealed that all three family members had elevated norepinephrine levels, indicating adrenal stress, which fueled anger and impulsivity. Mom's carbohydrate cravings were due to low serotonin levels, while Dad and Gabe had almost identically low dopamine levels. For Dad, low dopamine impacted his libido, while for Gabe, it resulted in ADD. Restoring her serotonin levels helped Mom reduce her carb cravings, and lose weight. Dopamine support (with 1-tyrosine) helped Dad's libido return. Following treatment with 5HTP, phosphatidyl serine, 1-theanine and later 1-tyrosine, Gabe's increased serotonin and reduced norepinephrine levels stabilized his mood, significantly improving his behavior and focus. Even his grades improved. Finally, reducing excitatory neurotransmitter levels for all family members reduced reactivity, impulsivity, and anger. The yelling and screaming stopped. After three months, Barbara Lester reported back to me that, "We're no longer overwhelmed by stress. This is the best thing that ever happened to us. Pam, you helped save my family." With our comprehensive approach, neither practitioners nor patients need ever be on their own. Via clear instructions, supplements, and prompts about when to transition and re-test, patients can become more self-assured. Practitioners are trained and are always in the loop, able to adjust their patients' care and address any problems. "After using the Neurogistics Brain Wellness Program with my clients, I now view Neurotransmitter Balancing as a foundational requirement for most individuals seeking to improve their health and well-being. My practitioner group relies on this program for successful patient outcomes," says William L. Wolcott, recognized worldwide as a leading authority in Metabolic Typing. "Running two multi-functional clinics for twenty-five years, I'd long considered brain chemistry a missing link in integrative health care. In the last year, since I incorporated NT treatment, my patients have finally received treatment for key health problems that have troubled them for years. The results are fantastic, and in some instances, nothing short of miraculous. I've seen undiagnosed bipolars' experience mood stabilization for the first time in their lives." John Franzi, DC, Metabolic Typing Advisor, Australia. Based on my experience and successful outcomes with even difficult cases, I have no doubt that NT balancing works. However, to achieve optimal results, it must be properly implemented. I offer these following guidelines for obtaining a comprehensive approach and ensuring that your patients reap the benefits of Neurotransmitter Balancing: * Determine actual NT levels via an objective measurement; I'm one of 3,500 US practitioners who consider Neurotransmitter Urinanalysis the best noninvasive test currently available. * Beware of simple checklists/questionnaires to replace analytical testing, as they only reveal a partial and often inaccurate view of the brain chemistry. Don't settle for anything less than * precisely customized protocols incorporating complete patient history and NT test results, * solid clinical experience backing all recommendations, * supplementation based on clinically derived protocols, and * patient follow-up to ensure compliance and success. Neurogistics also offers training and support to practitioners worldwide who want to integrate this modality into their practice, making it easier to achieve successful outcomes while learning and growing with this expanding field of treatment. Patients are also welcomed to access our services through our website program. For more information, please visit: www.Neurogistics.com or call (Practitioner) Toll-free: 877-801-8076 (Patient) Toll-free: 888-257-9068 Pam Machemehl Helmly, CN, is a graduate of Texas A & M University with a degree in Scientific Nutrition, She has worked in the health care industry since 1981. In 1997, she began to specialize in NT balancing working with industry pioneers. Since 2005, serving as Chief Science Officer of Neurogistics, Pam has been a leader in the field of changing the brain to improve mood, focus, enhance sports performance, and aid in the treatment of eating disorders. Neurogistics was founded by Pam Machemehl Helmly, CN, and marketing and business expert, Carla Roberts, Neurogistics offers a comprehensive NT balancing program. Our turnkey service includes a synthesis of objective testing results, along with subjective self-reports via a proprietary logic with results customized and reviewed by a clinical team. We provide diagnostics, treatment protocols, practitioner reports, patient reports, prompts to retest, and supplements to encourage compliance, follow-through, troubleshooting, and ongoing maintenance. Notes 1. Elenkov IJ, Wilder RL, Chrousos GP, Vizi ES. The sympathetic nerve--an integrative interface between two supersystems: The brain and the immune system. Pharmacol Rev. 2000 Dec;52(4):595-638. 2. Pliszka SR, McCracken JT, Maas JW. Catecholamines in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Current perspectives. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996 Mar;35(3):264-7. 3. David R. Cross and Karyn B. Purvis. "Neurotransmitter Profiles and Behavior Problems in a Sample of Special Needs Adopted Children." Texas Christian University, March 2005. by Pam Machemehl Helmly, CN COPYRIGHT 2007 The Townsend Letter Group COPYRIGHT 2007 Gale Group

Labels: , , , , , , ,

Thursday, June 28, 2007

Neuroarcheology of Childhood Trauma

Child Trauma Academy Home Child Trauma Academy Materials Child Trauma Academy Materials Child Trauma Academy Materials About Child Trauma Academy Child Trauma Academy Services Child Trauma Academy Materials Our Impact Resources & Links Forum The Neuroarcheology of Childhood Maltreatment The Neurodevelopmental Costs of Adverse Childhood Events Bruce D. Perry, M.D., Ph.D. The ChildTrauma Academy www.ChildTrauma.org For: "The Cost of Child Maltreatment: Who Pays? We All Do" (Ed., B. Geffner) Haworth Press July 27, 2000 Introduction Childhood maltreatment has profound impact on the emotional, behavioral, cognitive, social and physical functioning of children. Developmental experiences determine the organizational and functional status of the mature brain and, therefore, adverse events can have a tremendous negative impact on the development of the brain. In turn, these neurodevelopmental effects may result in significant cost to the individual, their family, community and, ultimately, society. In essence, childhood maltreatment alters the potential of a child and, thereby, robs us all. The present chapter will review some of those costs from a neurodevelopmental perspective. The premise is that when the core principles of neurodevelopment are understood, the costs of adverse childhood events and maltreatment become obvious. Following a brief presentation of the key concepts of neurodevelopment, two primary forms of maltreatment will be considered: (1) neglect and (2) traumatic stress. Maltreatment of children often involves both neglect and trauma; a more complete understanding of the complex neurodevelopmental impact of the combination, however, is best understood after presenting the potential effects of each separately. This chapter presents the current articulation of a neurodevelopmental perspective of childhood maltreatment originally outlined in 1994 (Perry. 1994) and further elaborated over the last five years (Perry, Pollard, Blakley, Baker, & Vigilante. 1995) (Perry & Pollard. 1998) This most recent articulation outlines the issue of maltreatment through the lens of developmental neurobiology and coins a descriptive phrase, "neuroarcheology," to capture the impact of adverse events on the developing brain, with the implicit suggestion that experiences leave a 'record' within the matrix of the brain. The nature and location of this record will depend upon the nature of the experience and the time in development when the event took place – much as with the archeological record of the earth. While this phrase may be simplistic to some, it conveys important conceptual principles about the nature of childhood experience which have been lacking all too often in clinical and research formulations regarding maltreatment. Not a single psychometric instrument measuring traumatic or adverse events, for example, uses time of trauma as a meaningful variable despite the fact that it may be the most important determinant of functional outcome following maltreatment. The neuroarcheological perspective on childhood experience, therefore, simply posits that the impact of a childhood event (adverse or positive) will be a reflection of (1) the nature, intensity, pattern and duration of the event and (2) that the resulting strengths (e.g., language) or deficits (e.g., neuropsychiatric symptoms) will be in those functions mediated by the neural systems that are most rapidly organizing (i.e., in the developmental "hot zone") at the time of the experience. Brain Organization and Function The human brain is the remarkable organ that allows us to sense, process, perceive, store and act on information from outside and inside the body to carry out the three prime directives required for the survival of our species: (1) survive, (2) affiliate and mate and then, (3) protect and nurture dependents. In order to carry out these core and overarching responsibilities, thousands of inter-related functions have evolved. In the human brain, structure and function have co-evolved. As we have a hierarchy of increasingly complex functions related to our optimal functioning, our brain has evolved a hierarchical structural organization (see Table 1). This hierarchy starts with the lower, simpler brainstem areas and increases in complexity up through the neocortex (Figure 1). In each of these many areas of the brain are neural systems that mediate our many brain-related functions (Figure1; Table1). The 'lower' parts of the brain (brainstem and midbrain) mediate simpler regulatory functions (e.g., regulation of respiration, heart rate, blood pressure, body temperature) while more complex functions (e.g., language and abstract thinking) are mediated by the more complex neocortical structures of the human brain. This hierarchical structure is the heart of a neuroarcheological understanding of adverse childhood events. This structure becomes the multi-layered soil within which the fossilized evidence of maltreatment can be found – each layer organizing at a different time and each layer reflecting the experiences –good and bad - of that era in the individual's life. Key insights to understanding human functioning, then, will come from understanding neurodevelopment. neuroarcheology1.jpg (33179 bytes) Figure 1: Hierarchical Organization of the Human Brain: The brain can be divided into four interconnected areas: brainstem, diencephalons, limbic and neocortex. The complexity of structure, cellular organization and function increases from the lower, simpler areas such as the brainstem to the most complex, the neocortex. Neurodevelopment Our brain's complex structure is comprised of 100 billion neurons and ten times as many glial cells – all interconnected by trillions of synaptic connections – and communicating in a non-stop, ever-changing dynamic of neurochemical activity. The brain doesn't just pop into existence. This most complex of all biological systems in the known universe is a product of neurodevelopment – a long process orchestrating billions upon billions of complex chemical transactions. It is through these chemical actions that a human being is created. The developing child is a remarkable phenomenon of nature. In a few short years, one single cell – the fertilized egg – becomes a walking, talking, learning, loving, and thinking being. This physical transformation is equivalent to a 6-foot tall, 200 pound man growing to the size of Connecticut in three years. In each of the billions and billions of cells in the body, a single set of genes has been expressed in millions of different combinations with precise timing. Development is a breathtaking orchestration of precision micro-construction that allows the healthy development of a human being. And the most remarkable and complex of all the organs in the human body is the human brain. In order to create the brain, a small set of pre-cursor cells must divide, move, specialize, connect and create specialized neural networks that form functional units. The key processes in neurodevelopment are summarized below. Core Processes of Neurodevelopment 1. Neurogenesis: The brain starts as a few cells present early in the first weeks of life. From a few specialized cells in the unformed brain, come billions of nerve cells and trillions of glia. This, of course, requires that cells be "born." Neurogenesis is the birth of new neurons. The vast majority of neurogenesis takes place in utero during the second and third trimester. At birth, the vast majority of neurons, literally more than 100 billion, used for the remainder of life are present. Few neurons are born after birth, although researchers have demonstrated recently that neurogenesis can and does take place in the mature brain (Gould, Reeves, Graziano, & Gross. 1999). This is a very significant observation and may be one of the important physiological mechanisms responsible for the brain's plasticity (i.e., capacity to restore function) following injury. Despite being present at birth, these neurons have yet to organize into completely functional systems. Many neurons need to mature themselves and undergo a set of processes that create the functional neural networks of the mature brain (Table 2). 2. Migration: Developing neurons move. Often guided by glial cells and a variety of chemical markers (e.g., cellular adhesion molecules, nerve growth factor: NGF), neurons cluster, sort, move and settle into a location in the brain that will be their final "resting" place. It is the fate of some neurons to settle in the brainstem, others in the cortex, for example. More than one half of all neurons are in the cortex. The processes of cortical cell migration and fate mapping are some of the most studied in all of developmental neuroscience (Rakic. 1981) (Rakic. 1996). It is clear that both genetic and environmental factors play important roles in determining a neuron's final location. Migration takes place primarily during the intrauterine and immediate perinatal period but continues throughout childhood and, possibly, to some degree into adult life. A host of intrauterine and perinatal insults – including infection, lack of oxygen, alcohol and various psychotropic drugs can alter migration of neurons and have profound impact on functioning (Perry. 1988). Table 1. A Neuroarcheological Chart of Development: Functional Organization neuroarcheology2.jpg (63967 bytes) 3. Differentiation: Neurons mature. Each of the 100 billion neurons in the brain has the same set of genes, yet each neuron is expressing a unique combination of those genes to create a unique identity. Some neurons are large, with long axons; others short. Neurons can mature to use any of a hundred different neurotransmitters such as norepinephrine, dopamine, serotonin, CRF or substance P. Neurons can have dense dendritic fields receiving input from hundreds of other neurons, while other neurons can have a single linear input from one other neuron. Each of these thousands of differentiating "choices" come as a result of the pattern, intensity and timing of various microenvironmental cues which tell the neuron to turn on some genes and turn off others. Each neuron undergoes a series of "decisions" to determine their final location and specialization. These decisions, again, are a combination of genetic and microenvironmental cues. The further along in development, the more differentiated the neuron, the more sensitive it becomes to the environmental signals. From the intrauterine period through early childhood (and to some degree beyond) neurons are very sensitive to experience-based signals, many of which are mediated by patterned neuronal activity in the neural network in which they reside. Neurons are literally designed to change in response to chemical signals. Therefore, any experience or event that alters these neurochemical or microenvironmental signals during development can change the ways in which certain neurons differentiate, thereby altering the functional capacity of the neural networks in which these neurons reside. 4. Apoptosis: Some developing neurons die. In many areas of the brain, there are more neurons born than are needed for any given function. Many of these neurons are redundant and when unable to adequately "connect" into an active neural network will die (Kuan, Roth, Flavell, & Rakic. 2000). Research in this area suggests that these neurons may play a role in the remarkable flexibility present in the human brain at birth. Depending upon the challenges of the environment and the potential needs of the individual, some neurons will survive while others will not. Again, this process appears to have genetic and environmental determinants. Neurons that make synaptic connections with others and have an adequate level of activation will survive; those cells that have little activity resorb. This is one example of a general principle of activity-dependence ("use it or lose it") that appears to be important in many neural processes related to learning, memory and development. 5. Arborization: As neurons differentiate, they send out tiny fiber-like extensions from their cell body. These dendrites become the receptive area where other neurons connect. It is in this receptive field that dozens to hundreds of other neurons are able to send neurochemical signals to the neuron. The density of these dendritic branches appears to be related to the frequency and intensity of incoming signals. When there is high activity, the dendritic network extends, essentially branching out in the same fashion as a bush may create new branches. This arborization allows the neuron to receive, process and integrate complex patterns of activity that will, in turn, determine its activity. Again, the arborization process appears to be to some degree activity-dependent. The density of the dendritic arborization appears to be related to the complexity and activity of incoming neural activity. In turn, these neural signals are often dependent upon the complexity and activity of the environment of the animal (Diamond, Law, Rhodes, et al. 1966; Greenough, Volkmar, & Juraska. 1973). 6. Synaptogenesis: Developing neurons make connections with each other. The major mechanism for neuron-to-neuron communication is 'receptor-mediated' neurotransmission that takes place at specialized connections between neurons called synapses. At the synapse, the distance between two neurons is very short. A chemical (classified as a neurotransmitter, neuromodulator or neurohormone) is released from the 'presynaptic' neuron and into the extra-cellular space (called the synaptic cleft) and binds to a specialized receptor protein in the membrane of the 'postsynaptic' neuron. By occupying the binding site, the neurotransmitter helps change the shape of this receptor which then catalyzes a secondary set of chemical interactions inside the postsynaptic neuron that create second messengers. The second messengers such as cyclic AMP, inositol phosphate and calcium will then shift the intracellular chemical milieu which may even influence the activity of specific genes. This cascade of intracellular chemical responses allows communication from one neuron to another. A continuous dynamic of synaptic neurotransmission regulates the activity and functional properties of the chains of neurons that allow the brain to do all of its remarkable activities. These neural connections are not random. They are guided by important genetic and environmental cues. In order for our brain to function properly, neurons, during development, need to find and connect with the "right" neurons. During the differentiation process, neurons send fiber-like projections (growth cones) out to make physical contact with other neurons. This process appears to be regulated and guided by certain growth factors and cellular adhesion molecules that attract or repel a specific growth cone to appropriate target neurons. Depending upon a given neuron's specialization, these growth cones will grow (becoming axons) and connect to the dendrites of other cells and create a synapse. During the first eight months of life there is an eight-fold increase in synaptic density while the developing neurons in the brain are "seeking" their appropriate connections (Huttenlocher. 1979) (Huttenlocher. 1994). This explosion of synaptogenesis allows the brain to have the flexibility to organize and function in with a wide range of potential. It is over the next few years, in response to patterned repetitive experiences that these neural connections will be refined and sculpted. 7. Synaptic sculpting: The synapse is a dynamic structure. With ongoing episodic release of neurotransmitter, occupation of receptors, release of growth factors, shifts of ions in and out of cells, laying down of new microtubules and other structural molecules, the synapse is continually changing. A key determinant of change in the synapse appears to be the level of presynaptic activity. When there is a consistent active process of neurotransmitter release, synaptic connections will be strengthened with actual physical changes that make the pre- and postsynaptic neurons come closer and the process of neurotransmission more efficient. When there is little activity, the synaptic connection will literally dissolve. The specific axonal branch to a given neuron will go away. Again, this powerful activity-dependent process appears to be very important for understanding learning, memory and the development. At any given moment – all throughout life – we are making and breaking synaptic connections. For the majority of life we are at equilibrium; the rate of creating new synaptic connections is equal to the rate of resorbing older, unused connections. While somewhat simplistic, it appears that the synaptic sculpting is a "use it or lose it" process. During the first eight months following birth the rate of creating new synapses far outstrips the rate of resorbing unused connections. By age one, however, and from then through early childhood, the rate of resorbing new connections is faster than the rate of creating new synapses. By adolescence, in most cortical areas at least, this process again reaches equilibrium. 8. Myelination: Specialized glial cells wrap around axons and, thereby, create more efficient electrochemical transduction down the neuron. This allows a neural network to function more rapidly and efficiently, thereby allowing more complex functioning (e.g., walking depends upon the myelination of neurons in the spinal cord for efficient, smooth regulation of neuromotor functioning.) The process of myelination begins in the first year of life but continues in many key areas throughout childhood with a final burst of myelination in key cortical areas taking place in adolescence. Table 2: Key Processes in Neurodevelopment neuroarcheology3.jpg (68908 bytes) * This refers to the age at which approximately 10% of this specific function is taking place. In most cases, there is evidence that some of these processes have started to some degree. Almost all of these processes continue in some form throughout life, the table is designed to illustrate the relative importance of childhood for the majority of activity in each of these processes. **These are crude estimates based upon data from multiple sources. The major point it to demonstrate that shifting activity from neurogenesis to myelination. All of the neurodevelopmental processes described above are dependent upon both genetic and environmentally determined microenvironmental cues (e.g., neurotransmitters, neuromodulators, neurohormones, ions, growth factors, cellular adhesion molecules and other morphogens). Disruption of the pattern, timing or intensity of these cues can lead to abnormal neurodevelopment and profound dysfunction. The neuroarcheological perspective suggests that the specific dysfunction will depend upon the timing of the insult (e.g., was the insult in utero during the development of the brainstem or at age two during the active development of the cortex), the nature of the insult (e.g., is there a lack of sensory stimulation from neglect or an abnormal persisting activation of the stress response from trauma?), the pattern of the insult (i.e., is this a discreet single event, a chronic experience with a chaotic pattern or an episodic event with a regular pattern?). While we are only beginning to understand the complexity of neurodevelopment, there are several key principles that emerge from the thousands of studies and years of focused research on these neurodevelopmental processes. These principles, as outlined below, suggest that while the structural organization and functional capabilities of the mature brain can change throughout life, the majority of the key stages of neurodevelopment take place in childhood. The core principles of neurodevelopment that support a neuroarcheological perspective of childhood adverse events are summarized below. Core Principles of Neurodevelopment 1. Nature and nurture: For too many years, any conceptual approach to human behavior has been tainted by the nature versus nurture debate. Do genes cause human behavior or is human behavior a product of learning, education and experience? Ultimately, this debate polarizes and distracts from more complex understandings of human functioning. Genes are designed to work in an environment. Genes are expressed by microenvironmental cues, which, in turn, are influenced by the experiences of the individual. How an individual functions within an environment, then, is dependent upon the expression of a unique combination of genes available to the human species. We don't have the genes to make wings. And what we become depends upon how experiences shape the expression – or not - of specific genes we do have. We do have the genes to make forty sounds – and we can have the experiences that turn this genetically determined capacity into a powerful, transforming tool – language. Yet, there are many sad examples of cruel experiments of humanity, where a young child was raised in an environment deprived of language. This child, despite the genetic potential to speak and think and feel in complex humane ways, did not express that potential fully. Genetic potential without appropriately timed experiences can remain unexpressed. Nature and nurture – we are nothing without both; we require both and we are products of both. The influence of gene-driven processes, however, shifts during development. In the just fertilized ovum, all of the chemical processes that are driving development are very dependent upon a genetically determined sequence of molecular events. By birth, however, the brain has developed to the point where environmental cues mediated by the senses play a major role in determining how neurons will differentiate, sprout dendrites, form and maintain synaptic connections and create the final neural networks that convey functionality. By adolescence, the majority of the changes that are taking place in the brain of that child are determined by experience, not genetics. The languages, beliefs, cultural practices, and complex cognitive and emotional functioning (e.g., self esteem) by this age are primarily experience-based. 2. Sequential Developmental: The brain develops in a sequential and hierarchical fashion; organizing itself from least (brainstem) to most complex (limbic, cortical areas). These different areas develop, organize and become fully functional at different times during childhood. At birth, for example, the brainstem areas responsible for regulating cardiovascular and respiratory function must be intact for the infant to survive, and any malfunction is immediately observable. In contrast, the cortical areas responsible for abstract cognition have years before they will be 'needed' or fully functional. This means that each brain area will have its own timetable for development. The neurodevelopmental processes described above will be most active in different brain areas at different times and will, therefore, either require (critical periods) or be sensitive to (sensitive periods) organizing experiences (and the neurotrophic cues related to these experiences). The neurons for the brainstem have to migrate, differentiate and connect, for example, before the neurons for the cortex. The implications of this for a neuroarcheological formulation are profound. Disruptions of experience-dependent neurochemical signals during these periods may lead to major abnormalities or deficits in neurodevelopment. Disruption of critical neurodevelopmental cues can result from 1) lack of sensory experience during sensitive periods (e.g., neglect) or 2) atypical or abnormal patterns of necessary cues due to extremes of experience (e.g., traumatic stress, see below). Insults during the intrauterine period, for example, will more likely influence the rapidly organizing brainstem systems as opposed to the more slowly organizing cortical areas. The symptoms from the intrauterine disruption will alter functions mediated by the brainstem and could include sensory integration problems, hyper-reactivity, poor state regulation (e.g., sleep, feeding, self-soothing), tactile defensiveness and altered regulation of core neurophysiological functions such as respiration, cardiovascular and temperature regulation. This does not mean that neocortical systems are unaffected by disrupting the development of the brainstem. Indeed, one of the most important aspects of the sequential development is that important organizing signals for any given brain area or system (e.g., patterns of neural activity, neurotransmitters acting as morphogens) come from previously organized brain areas or systems. Due to the sequential development of the brain, disruptions of normal developmental processes early in life (e.g., during the perinatal period) that alter development of the brainstem or diencephalon will necessarily alter the development of limbic and cortical areas. This is so because many of the organizing cues for normal limbic and neocortical organization originate in the lower brain areas. Any developmental insult can have a cascade effect on the development of all "downstream" brain areas (and functions) that will receive input from the effected neural system. 3. Activity-dependent neurodevelopment: The brain organizes in a use-dependent fashion. As described above, many of the key processes in neurodevelopment are activity dependent. In the developing brain, undifferentiated neural systems are critically dependent upon sets of environmental and micro-environmental cues (e.g., neurotransmitters, cellular adhesion molecules, neurohormones, amino acids, ions) in order for them to appropriately organize from their undifferentiated, immature forms (Lauder. 1988; Perry. 1994) (Perry & Pollard. 1998). Lack, or disruption, of these critical cues can alter the neurodevelopmental processes of neurogenesis, migration, differentiation, synaptogenesis - all of which can contribute to malorganization and diminished functional capabilities in the specific neural system where development has been disrupted. This is the core of a neuroarcheological perspective on dysfunction related adverse childhood events (Perry. 1994) (Perry & Pollard. 1998; Perry. 1998). These molecular cues that guide development are dependent upon the experiences of the developing child. The quantity, pattern of activity and nature of these neurochemical and neurotrophic factors depends upon the presence and the nature of the total sensory experience of the child. When the child has adverse experiences – loss, threat, neglect, and injury – there can be disruptions of neurodevelopment that will result in neural organization that can lead to compromised functioning throughout life (see Neglect section, below). A neuroarcheological perspective would predict that the dysfunction resulting from a specific adverse event is related to the disrupted (or altered) development of the neural system that is, during the adverse event, most rapidly developing. The degree of disruption is related to the rate of change in the respective neural system. The already organized and functioning neural system is less vulnerable to a developmental insult than the rapidly changing, energy-hungry and microenvironmental cue-sensitive developing system. This is so because of a principle called biological relativity. In any dynamic system, the impact of an event or experience (disruptive or positive) is greatest on the most actively changing or dynamic parts of that system. The power of any experience, therefore, is greatest during the most rapid phases of development. Events taking place during a neural system's most active phase of organization will have more impact than events after the system has organized. 4. Windows of Opportunity/Windows of Vulnerability: The sequential development of the brain and the activity-dependence of many key aspects of neurodevelopment suggest that there must be times during development when a given developing neural system is more sensitive to experience than others (Table 3). In healthy development, that sensitivity allows the brain to rapidly and efficiently organize in response to the unique demands of a given environment to express from its broad genetic potential those characteristics which best fit that child's world. If the child speaks Japanese as opposed to English, for example, or if this child will live in the plains of Africa or the tundra of the Yukon, different genes can be expressed, different neural networks can be organized from that child's potential to best fit that family, culture and environment. We all are aware of how rapidly young children can learn language, develop new behaviors and master new tasks. The very same neurodevelopmental sensitivity that allows amazing developmental advances in response to predictable, nurturing, repetitive and enriching experiences make the developing child vulnerable to adverse experiences. Sensitive periods are different for each brain area and neural system, and therefore, for different functions. The sequential development of the brain and the sequential unfolding of the genetic map for development mean that the sensitive periods for neural system (and the functions they mediate) will be when that system is in the developmental 'hot zone' – when that area is most actively organizing. The brainstem must organize key systems by birth; therefore, the sensitive period for those brainstem-mediated functions is during the prenatal period. The neocortex, in contrast, has systems and functions organizing throughout childhood and into adult life. The sensitive periods for these cortically mediated functions are likely to be very long. With an understanding of the shifting vulnerability of the developing brain to experience, a neuroarcheological perspective becomes apparent. If there are disrupting adverse events during development, they will be mirrored by a matched dysfunctional development in the neural systems whose functioning the adverse experience most altered during the event. If the disruption were the absence of light during the first year of life – the systems most altered would be related to vision. If the disruption activates the stress response, the disruption will be in the neural systems mediating the stress response. The severity and chronicity of the specific dysfunction will be related to the vulnerability of the system affected. Adverse experiences influence the mature brain but in the developing brain, adverse experiences literally play a role in organizing neural systems. It is much easier to influence the functioning of a developing system than to reorganize and alter the functioning of a developed system. Adverse childhood events, therefore, can alter the organization of developing neural systems in ways that create a lifetime of vulnerability. Table 3: Shifting Developmental Activity across Brain Regions neuroarcheology4.jpg (35426 bytes) The simple and unavoidable conclusion of these neurodevelopmental principles is that the organizing, sensitive brain of an infant or young children is more malleable to experience than a mature brain. While experience may alter the behavior of an adult, experience literally provides the organizing framework for an infant and child. Because the brain is most plastic (receptive to environmental input) in early childhood, the child is most vulnerable to variance of experience during this time. In the second half of this chapter two primary forms of extreme childhood adverse experience will be discussed in context of the neuroarcheological perspective of adverse childhood events. The Neurodevelopmental Impact of Neglect in Childhood Neglect is the absence of critical organizing experiences at key times during development. Despite its obvious importance in understanding child maltreatment, neglect has been understudied. Indeed, deprivation of critical experiences during development may be the most destructive yet the least understood area of child maltreatment. There are several reasons for this. The most obvious is that neglect is difficult to "see." Unlike a broken bone, maldevelopment of neural systems mediating empathy, for example, resulting from emotional neglect during infancy, is not readily observable. Another important, yet poorly appreciated, aspect of neglect is the issue of timing. The needs of the child shift during development; therefore, what may be neglectful at one age is not at another. The very same experience that is essential for life at one stage of life may be of little significance or even inappropriate at another age. We would all question the mother who held, rocked and breastfed her pubescent child. Touch, for example, is essential during infancy. The untouched newborn may literally die; in Spitz' landmark studies, the mortality rates in the institutionalized infants was near thirty percent (Spitz. 1945; Spitz. 1946). If one doesn't touch an adolescent for weeks, however, no significant adverse effects will result. Creating standardized protocols, procedures and "measures" of neglect, therefore, are significantly confounded by the shifting developmental needs and demands of childhood. Finally, neglect is understudied because it is very difficult to find large populations of humans where specific and controlled neglectful experiences have been well documented. In some cases, these cruel experiments of humanity have provided unique and promising insights (see below). In general, however, there will never be – and there never should be – the opportunity to study neglect in humans with the rigor that can be applied in animal models. With these limitations, however, what we do know about neglect during early childhood supports a neuroarcheological view of adverse childhood experience. The earlier and more pervasive the neglect is, the more devastating the developmental problems for the child. Indeed, a chaotic, inattentive and ignorant caregiver can produce pervasive developmental delay (PDD; (Anonymous. 1994)) in a young child (Rutter, Andersen-Wood, Beckett, et al. 1999). Yet the very same inattention for the same duration if the child is ten will have very different and less severe impact than inattention during the first years of life. There are two main sources of insight to childhood neglect. The first is the indirect but more rigorous animal studies and the second is a growing number of descriptive reports with severely neglected children. Environmental Manipulation and Neurodevelopment: Animal Studies Some of the most important studies in developmental neurosciences in the last century have been focusing on various aspects of experience and extreme sensory experience models. Indeed, the Nobel Prize was awarded to Hubel and Weisel for their landmark studies on development of the visual system using sensory deprivation techniques (Hubel & Wiesel. 1963). In hundreds of other studies, extremes of sensory deprivation (Hubel & Wiesel. 1970; Greenough, Volkmar, & Juraska. 1973) or sensory enrichment (Greenough & Volkmar. 1973; Diamond, Krech, & Rosenzweig. 1964; Diamond, Law, Rhodes, et al. 1966) have been studied. These include disruptions of visual stimuli (Coleman & Riesen. 1968), environmental enrichment (Altman & Das. 1964; Cummins & Livesey. 1979), touch (Ebinger. 1974; Rutledge, Wright, & Duncan. 1974), and other factors that alter the typical experiences of development (Uno, Tarara, Else, & et.al. 1989; Plotsky & Meaney. 1993; Meaney, Aitken, van Berkal, Bhatnagar, & Sapolsky. 1988). These findings generally demonstrate that the brains of animals reared in enriched environments are larger, more complex and functional more flexible than those raised under deprivation conditions. Diamond's work, for example, examining the relationships between experience and brain cytoarchitecture have demonstrated a relationship between density of dendritic branching and the complexity of an environment (for a good review of this and related data see (Diamond & Hopson. 1998)). Others have shown that rats raised in environmentally enriched environments have higher density of various neuronal and glial microstructures, including a 30% higher synaptic density in cortex compared to rats raised in an environmentally deprived setting (Bennett, Diamond, Krech, & Rosenzweig. 1964; Altman & Das. 1964). Animals raised in the wild have from 15 to 30% larger brain mass than their offspring who are domestically reared (Darwin. 1868; Rohrs. 1955; Rohrs & Ebinger. 1978; Rehkamper, Haase, & Frahm. 1988). Animal studies suggest that critical periods exist during which specific sensory experience was required for optimal organization and development of the part of the brain mediating a specific function (e.g., visual input during the development of the visual cortex). While these phenomena have been examined in great detail for the primary sensory modalities in animals, few studies have examined the issues of critical or sensitive periods in humans. What evidence there is would suggest that humans tend to have longer periods of sensitivity and that the concept of critical period may not be useful in humans. It is plausible, however, that abnormal micro-environmental cues and atypical patterns of neural activity during sensitive periods in humans could result in malorganization and compromised function in a host of brain-mediated functions. Indeed, altered emotional, behavioral, cognitive, social and physical functioning has been demonstrated in humans following specific types of neglect. The majority of this information comes from the clinical rather than the experimental disciplines. The Impact of Neglect in Early Childhood: Clinical Findings Over the last sixty years, many case reports, case series and descriptive studies have been conducted with children neglected in early childhood. The majority of these studies have focused on institutionalized children. As early as 1833, with the famous Kaspar Hauser, feral children had been described (Heidenreich. 1834). Hauser was abandoned as a young child and raised from early childhood (likely around age two) until seventeen in a dungeon, experiencing relative sensory, emotional and cognitive neglect. His emotional, behavioral and cognitive functioning was, as one might expect, very primitive and delayed. At autopsy, Hauser's brain was noted to have a small cerebrum (cortex) with few and non-distinct cortical gyri. These findings are consistent with cortical atrophy (or underdevelopment), a condition we have reported in children following severe total global neglect in childhood (Perry & Pollard. 1997). In the early forties, Spitz described the impact of neglectful caregiving on children in foundling homes (orphanages). Most significant, he was able to demonstrate that children raised in fostered placements with more attentive and nurturing caregiving had superior physical, emotional and cognitive outcomes (Spitz. 1945; Spitz. 1946). Some of the most powerful clinical examples of this phenomenon are related to profound neglect experiences early in life. In a landmark report of children raised in a Lebanese orphanage, the Creche, Dennis (1973) described a series of findings supporting a neuroarcheological model of maltreatment. These children were raised in an institutional environment devoid of individual attention, cognitive stimulation, emotional affection or other enrichment. Prior to 1956 all of these children remained at the orphanage until age six, at which time they were transferred to another institution. Evaluation of these children at age 16 demonstrated a mean IQ of approximately 50. When adoption became common, children adopted prior to age 2 had a mean IQ of 100 by adolescence while children adopted between ages 2 and 6 had IQ values of approximately 80 (Dennis. 1973). This graded recovery reflected the neuroarcheological impact of neglect. A number of similar studies of children adopted from neglectful settings demonstrate this general principle. The older a child was at time of adoption, (i.e., the longer the child spent in the neglectful environment) the more pervasive and resistant to recovery were the deficits. Money and Annecillo (1976) reported the impact of change in placement on children with psychosocial dwarfism (failure to thrive). In this preliminary study, 12 of 16 children removed from neglectful homes recorded remarkable increases in IQ and other aspects of emotional and behavioral functioning. Furthermore, they reported that the longer the child was out of the abusive home the higher the increase in IQ. In some cases IQ increased by 55 points (Money & Annecillo. 1976). A more recent report on a group of 111 Romanian orphans (Rutter & English and Romanian Adoptees study team. 1998; Rutter, Andersen-Wood, Beckett, et al. 1999) adopted prior to age two from very emotionally and physically depriving institutional settings demonstrate similar findings. Approximately one half of the children were adopted prior to age six months and the other half between six months and 2 years old. At the time of adoption, these children had significant delays. Four years after being placed in stable and enriching environments, these children were re-evaluated. While both groups improved, the group adopted at a younger age had a significantly greater improvement in all domains. These observations are consistent with the experiences of our clinic research group working with maltreated children. Over the last ten-year we have worked with more than 1000 children neglected in some fashion. We have recorded increases in IQ of over 40 points in more than 60 children following removal from neglectful environments and placed in consistent, predictable, nurturing, safe and enriching placements (Perry et al., in preparation). In addition, in a study of more than 200 children under the age of 6 removed from parental care following abuse and neglect we demonstrated significant developmental delays in more than 85% of the children. The severity of these developmental problems increased with age, suggesting, again, that the longer the child was in the adverse environment - the earlier and more pervasive the neglect - the more indelible and pervasive the deficits. The impact of deprivation can be approximated by sensory chaos. Indeed, sensory deprivation is much less clinically significant than sensory chaos. The vast majority of children suffering from neglect do so because their experiences are chaotic, dysynchronous, inconsistent and episodic rather than consistent, predictable and continuous. The organizing brain requires patterns of sensory experience to create patterns of neural activity that, in turn, play a role in guiding the various neurodevelopmental processes involved in healthy development. When experience is chaotic or sensory patterns are not consistent and predictable, the organizing systems in the brain reflect this chaos and, typically, organize in ways that result in dysregulation and dysynchronous. Imagine trying to learn a language if you only heard random words without the context, grammar and syntax of the language (i.e., the patterns of use). Even if you heard and perceived all words, you could not develop language. Random exposure to words absent an organizing pattern leads to abnormal development of speech and language. Our clinical group has evaluated many children capable of parroting advertising phrases from television but incapable of simple verbal communication. This requirement for consistent, repetitive and patterned stimuli holds for all experience – cognitive, emotional, social and physical. Repetitive, patterned, consistent experience allows the brain to create an internal representation of the external world. A child growing up in the midst of chaos and unpredictability will develop neural systems and functional capabilities that reflect this disorganization. The Impact of Neglect in Early Childhood: Neurobiological Findings All of these reported developmental problems – language, fine and large motor delays, impulsivity, disorganized attachment, dysphoria, attention and hyperactivity, and a host of others described in these neglected children – are caused by abnormalities in the brain. Despite this obvious statement, very few studies have examined directly any aspect of neurobiology in neglected children. The reasons include a lack of capacity, until the recent past, to examine the brain in any non-invasive fashion. Our group has examined various aspects of neurodevelopment in neglected children (Perry & Pollard. 1997). Neglect was considered global neglect when a history of relative sensory deprivation in more than one domain was obtained (e.g., minimal exposure to language, touch and social interactions). Chaotic neglect is far more common and was considered present if history was obtained that was consistent with physical, emotional, social or cognitive neglect. When possible history was obtained from multiple sources (e.g., investigating CPS workers, family, police). The neglected children (n= 122) were divided into four groups: Global Neglect (GN; n=40); Global Neglect with Prenatal Drug Exposure (GN+PND; n=18); Chaotic Neglect (CN; n=36); Chaotic Neglect with Prenatal Drug Exposure (CN+PND; n=28). Measures of growth were compared across group and compared to standard norms developed and used in all major pediatric settings. Dramatic differences from the norm were observed in FOC (the frontal-occipital circumference, a measure of head size and in young children a reasonable measure of brain size). In the globally neglected children the lower FOC values suggested abnormal brain growth. For these globally neglected children the group mean was below the 8th percentile. In contrast, the chaotically neglected children did not demonstrate this marked group difference in FOC. Furthermore in cases where MRI or CT scans were available, neuroradiologists interpreted 11 of 17 scans as abnormal from the children with global neglect (64.7 %) and only 3 of 26 scans abnormal from the children with chaotic neglect (11.5 %). The majority of the readings were "enlarged ventricles" or "cortical atrophy." While the actual size of the brain in chaotically neglected children did not appear to be different from norms, it is reasonable to hypothesize that organizational abnormalities exist and that with function MRI studies these abnormalities will be more readily detected. These findings strongly suggest that when early life neglect is characterized by decreased sensory input (e.g., relative poverty of words, touch and social interactions) there will be a similar effect on human brain growth as in other mammalian species. The human cortex grows in size, develops complexity, makes synaptic connections and modifies as a function of the quality and quantity of sensory experience. Lack of type and quantity of sensory-motor and cognitive experiences lead to underdevelopment of the cortex – in rats, non-human primates and humans. Studies from other groups are beginning to report similar altered neurodevelopment in neglected children. In the study of Romanian orphans described above, the 38 % had FOC values below the third percentile (greater than 2 SD from the norm) at the time of adoption. In the group adopted after six months, fewer than 3 % and the group adopted after six months 13 % had persistently low FOCs four years later (Rutter & English and Romanian Adoptees study team. 1998; O'Connor, Rutter, & English and Romanian Adoptees study team. 2000). Strathearn (Strathearn et al., submitted) has followed extremely low birth weight infants and shown that when these infants end up in neglectful homes they have a significantly smaller head circumference at 2 and 4 years, but not at birth. This is despite having no significant difference in other growth parameters. Finally in a related population, maltreated children and adolescents with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), De Bellis and colleagues found that subject children have significantly smaller intracranial and cerebral volumes than matched controls on MRI scan. Brain volume in these children correlated "robustly and positively" with the age of onset of PTSD trauma, and negatively with the duration of abuse, suggesting that traumatic childhood experiences may adversely affect brain development. Specific brain areas were affected differentially, in reflection of their importance in the stress response, further support of a neuroarcheological formulation of adverse childhood experience (De Bellis, Keshavan, Clark, et al. 1999). While deprivations and lack of specific sensory experiences are common in the maltreated child, the traumatized child experiences developmental insults related to discrete patterns of over-activation of neurochemical cues. Rather than a deprivation of sensory stimuli, the traumatized child experiences over-activation of important neural systems during sensitive periods of development. The Neurodevelopmental Impact of Traumatic Stress in Childhood Each year in United States more than five million children are exposed to some form of extreme traumatic stressor. These traumatic events include natural disasters (e.g., tornadoes, floods, hurricanes), motor vehicle accidents, life threatening illness and associated painful medical procedures (e.g., severe burns, cancer), physical abuse, sexual assault, witnessing domestic or community violence, kidnapping and sudden death of a parent, among others (Pfefferbaum. 1997; Anonymous. 1998). These events, posing an actual or perceived threat to the individual, activate a stress response. During the traumatic event, the child's brain mediates the adaptive response. Brainstem and diencehpalic stress-mediating neural systems are activated. These systems include the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, central nervous system (CNS) noradrenergic (NA), dopaminergic (DA) systems and associated CNS and peripheral systems that provide the adaptive emotional, behavioral, cognitive and physiological changes necessary for survival (Perry. 1994; Perry & Pollard. 1998). Individual neurobiological responses during traumatic stress are heterogeneous (Perry, Pollard, Blakley, Baker, & Vigilante. 1995). The specific nature of a child's responses to a given traumatic event may vary with the nature, duration and the pattern of traumatic stressor and the child's constitutional characteristics (e.g., genetic predisposition, age, gender, history of previous stress exposure, presence of attenuating factors such as supportive caregivers). Whatever the individual response, however, the extreme nature of the external threat is matched by an extreme and persisting internal activation of the neurophysiological systems mediating the stress response and their associated functions (Perry, Pollard, Blakley, Baker, & Vigilante. 1995; Perry & Pollard. 1998). As described above, neural systems respond to prolonged, repetitive activation by altering their neurochemical and sometimes, microarchitectural (e.g., synaptic sculpting) organization and functioning. This is no different for the neural systems mediating the stress response. Following any traumatic event children will likely experience some persisting emotional, behavioral, cognitive and physiological signs and symptoms related to the, sometimes temporary, shifts in the activity of these neural systems originating in the brainstem and diencephalon. In general, the longer the activation of the stress-response systems (i.e., the more intense and prolonged the traumatic event), the more likely there will be a 'use-dependent' change in these neural systems (for review see (Perry & Pollard. 1998)). In some cases, then, the stress-response systems do not return to the pre-event homeostasis. In these cases, the signs and symptoms become so severe, persisting and disruptive that they reach the level of a clinical disorder (Perry. 1998). In a new context and in the absence of any true external threat, the abnormal persistence of a once adaptive response becomes maladaptive. Post traumatic stress-related clinical syndromes Post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a clinical syndrome that may develop following extreme traumatic stress (DSM IV) (Anonymous. 1994). Like all other DSM IV diagnoses, it is likely that heterogeneous pathophysiologies underlie the cluster of diagnostic signs and symptoms labeled PTSD. There are six diagnostic criteria for PTSD: 1) extreme traumatic stress accompanied by intense fear, horror or disorganized behavior; 2) persistent re-experiencing of the traumatic event such as repetitive play or recurring intrusive thoughts; 3) avoidance of cues associated with the trauma or emotional numbing; 4) persistent physiological hyper-reactivity or arousal; 5) signs and symptoms present for more than one month following the traumatic event and 6) clinically significant disturbance in functioning. Posttraumatic stress disorder has been studied primarily in adult populations, most commonly combat veterans and victims of sexual assault. Despite high numbers of traumatized children, the clinical phenomenology, treatment and neurophysiological correlates of childhood PTSD remain under studied. The clinical phenomenology of trauma-related neuropsychiatric sequelae is poorly characterized (Terr. 1991; Mulder, Fergusson, Beautrais, & Joyce. 1998). Most of the studies of PTSD have been following single discreet trauma (e.g., a shooting). The least characterized populations are very young children and children with multiple or chronic traumatic events. Clinical presentations If during development, this stress response apparatus are required to be persistently active, the stress response apparatus in the central nervous system will develop in response to constant threat. These stress-response neural systems (and all functions they mediate – including sympathetic-parasympathetic tone, level of vigilance, regulation of mood, attention and sleep) will be poorly regulated, often overactive and hypersensitive. It is highly adaptive for a child growing up in a violent, chaotic environment to be hypersensitive to external stimuli, to be hypervigilant, and to be in a persistent stress-response state. It is important to realize that children exposed to traumatic stress during development literally organize their neural systems to adapt to this kind of environment. In contrast, an adult with no previous traumatic stress can develop PTSD. The cardiovascular reactivity and physiological hypersensitivity that the adult develops, however, is cue specific. This means that they will demonstrate increased heart rate, startle response and other neurophysiological symptoms when exposed to a cue from the original trauma (e.g., the Vietnam vet hearing a helicopter). In contrast, young children will develop a generalized physiological hyper-reactivity and hypersensitivity to all cues that activate the stress response apparatus. This generalized change results when the traumatic stress literally provides the organizing cues for their developing stress response neurobiology (Perry. 1999). Clinically, this is very easily seen in children who are exposed to chronic neurodevelopmental trauma. These children are frequently diagnosed as having attention deficit disorder (ADD-H) with hyperactivity (Haddad & Garralda. 1992). This is somewhat misleading, however. These children are hypervigilant; they do not have a core abnormality of their capacity to attend to a given task. These children have behavioral impulsivity, and cognitive distortions all of which result from a use-dependent organization of the brain (Perry, Pollard, Blakley, Baker, & Vigilante. 1995). During development, these children spent so much time in a low-level state of fear (mediated by brainstem and diencehpalic areas) that they consistently were focusing on non-verbal but not verbal cues. In our clinical population, children raised in chronically traumatic environments demonstrate a prominent V-P split on IQ testing (n = 108; WISC Verbal = 8. 2; WISC Performance = 10.4, Perry et al., in preparation). Often these children are labeled as learning disabled. We have seen these V-P splits in children in the juvenile justice system, child protective system and in the specialized clinical populations referred to our ChildTrauma clinic. These children are also characterized by persisting physiological hyperarousal and hyperactivity (Perry, Pollard, Baker, Sturges, Vigilante, & Blakley. 1995; Perry. 1994; Perry. 2000). These children are observed to have increased muscle tone, frequently a low grade increase in temperature, an increased startle response, profound sleep disturbances, affect regulation problems and anxiety (Kaufman. 1991; Ornitz & Pynoos. 1989; Perry. 2000). In addition, our studies indicate that a significant portion of these children have abnormalities in cardiovascular regulation (Perry, Pollard, Baker, Sturges, Vigilante, & Blakley. 1995; Perry. 2000). All of these symptoms are the result of a use-dependent organization of the brain stem nuclei involved in the stress response apparatus. Children with PTSD may present with a combination of problems including impulsivity, distractibility and attention problems (due to hypervigilance), dysphoria, emotional numbing, social avoidance, dissociation, sleep problems, aggressive (often re-enactment) play, school failure and regressed or delayed development. In most studies examining the development of PTSD following a given traumatic experience, twice as many children suffer from significant post-traumatic signs or symptoms (PTSS) but lack all of the criteria necessary for the diagnosis of PTSD (Friedrich. 1998). In these cases, the clinician may identify the trauma-related symptom as being part of another neuropsychiatric syndrome. The clinician is often unaware of ongoing traumatic stressors (e.g., domestic or community violence) or the family makes no association between the present symptoms and past events (e.g., car accident, death of a relative, exposure to violence) and may provide no relevant history to aid the clinician in the differential. As a result, PTSD is frequently misdiagnosed and PTSS are under recognized. Children with PTSD as a primary diagnosis are often labeled with Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity (ADHD), major depression, oppositional-defiant disorder, conduct disorder, separation anxiety or specific phobia. Ackerman and colleagues examined the prevalence of PTSD and other neuropsychiatric disorders in 204 abused children (ages 7 to 13) (Ackerman, Newton, McPHerson, Jones, & Dykman. 1998). Thirty four percent of these children met criteria for PTSD. Over fifty percent of the children in this study suffering both physical and sexual abuse had PTSD. Using structured diagnostic interview, the majority of these children met diagnostic criteria for three or more Axis I diagnoses in addition to PTSD. Indeed, only 6 of 204 children met criteria for only PTSD. The broad co-morbidity reported in this study echoes previous studies. Incidence and prevalence Children exposed to various traumatic events have much higher incidence (from 15 to 90+ %) and prevalence rates than the general population (Pfefferbaum. 1997). Furthermore, the younger a child is the more vulnerable they appear to be for the development of trauma-related symptoms. The percentage of children developing PTSD following a traumatic event is significantly higher than the percentage of adults developing PTSD following a similar traumatic stress. Several studies published in 1998 confirm previous reports of high prevalence rates for PTSD in child and adolescent populations. Thirty five percent of a sample of adolescents diagnosed with cancer met criteria for lifetime PTSD (Pelcovitz, Kaplan, Goldenberg, Mandel, Lehane, & Guarrera. 1994); 15 % of children surviving cancer had moderate to severe PTSS (Stuber, Kazak, Meeske, et al. 1997); 93 % of a sample of children witnessing domestic violence had PTSD (Kilpatrick & Williams. 1998); over 80 % of the Kuwaiti children exposed to the violence of the Gulf Crisis had PTSS (Hadi & Llabre. 1998); 73 % of juvenile male rape victims develop PTSD (Ruchkin, Eisemann, & Hagglof. 1998); 34 % of a sample of children experiencing sexual or physical abuse and 58 % of children experiencing both physical and sexual abuse all met criteria for PTSD (Ackerman, Newton, McPHerson, Jones, & Dykman. 1998). In all of these studies, clinically significant symptoms, though not full PTSD, were observed in essentially all of the children or adolescents following the traumatic experiences. Vulnerability and resilience Not all children exposed to traumatic events develop PTSD. A major research focus has been identifying factors (mediating factors) that are associated with increased (vulnerability) or decreased (resilience) risk for developing PTSD following exposure to traumatic stress (Kilpatrick & Williams. 1998). Factors previously demonstrated to be related to risk can be summarized in these broad categories: 1) characteristics of the child (e.g., subjective perception of threat to life or limb, history of previous traumatic exposures, coping style, general level of anxiety, gender, age); 2) characteristics of the event (e.g., nature of the event, direct physical harm, proximity to threat, pattern and duration); 3) characteristics of family/social system (e.g., supportive, calm, nurturing vs. chaotic, distant, absent, anxious) (Briggs & Joyce. 1997; Stuber, Kazak, Meeske, et al. 1997; Winje & Ulvik. 1998). Each of these mediating factors can be related to the degree to which they either prolong or attenuate the child's stress-response activation resulting from the traumatic experience. Factors that increase stress-related reactivity (e.g., family chaos) will make children more vulnerable while factors that provide structure, predictability, nurturing and sense of safety will decrease vulnerability. Persistently activated stress-response neurophysiology in the dependent, fearful child will predispose to a 'use-dependent' changes in the neural systems mediated the stress response, thereby resulting in post-traumatic stress symptoms (see Table 4). Table 4. Post-traumatic Stress Disorder: Risk and Attenuating Factors neuroarcheology5.jpg (94803 bytes) Long-term costs of childhood trauma PTSD is a chronic disorder. Untreated, PTSS and PTSD remit at a very low rate. Indeed the residual emotional, behavioral, cognitive and social sequelae of childhood trauma persist and appear to contribute to a host of neuropsychiatric problems throughout life (Fergusson & Horwood. 1998) including attachment problems (Bell & Belicki. 1998; Alexander, Anderson, Brand, Schaeffer, Grelling, & Kretz. 1998), eating disorders (Rorty & Yager. 1996), depression (Winje & Ulvik. 1998; Fergusson & Horwood. 1998), suicidal behavior (Molnar, Shade, Kral, Booth, & Watters. 1998), anxiety (Fergusson & Horwood. 1998), alcoholism (Fergusson & Horwood. 1998; Epstein, Saunders, Kilpatrick, & Resnick. 1998), violent behavior (O'Keefe. 1995), mood disorders (Kaufman. 1991) and, of course, PTSD (Ford & Kidd. 1998; Schaaf & McCanne. 1998). Childhood trauma impacts other aspects of physical health throughout life, as well (Hertzman & Wiens. 1996; Orr, Lasko, Metzger, Berry, Ahern, & Pitman. 1998; Felliti, Anda, Nordenberg, et al. 1998). Adults victimized by sexual abuse in childhood are more likely to have difficulty in childbirth, a variety of gastrointestinal and gynecological disorders and other somatic problems such as chronic pain, headaches and fatigue (Rhodes & Hutchinson. 1994). The Adverse Childhood Experiences study (Felliti, Anda, Nordenberg, et al. 1998) examined exposure to seven categories of adverse events during childhood (e.g., sexual abuse, physical abuse, witnessing domestic violence: events associated with increase risk for PTSD). This study found a graded relationship between the number of adverse events in childhood and the adult health and disease outcomes examined (e.g., heart disease, cancer, chronic lung disease, and various risk behaviors). With four or more adverse childhood events, the risk for various medical conditions increased 4- to 12-fold. Clearly studies of this sort will help clarify the true costs of childhood maltreatment. Summary and Future Directions The remarkable property of the human brain, unlike any other animal species, is that it has the capacity to take the accumulated experience of thousands of previous generations and absorb it within one lifetime. This capability is endowed by the design of our neural systems. Neurons and neural systems are designed to change in response to microenvironmental events. In turn, our experiences influence the pattern and nature of these microenvironmental signals, allowing neural systems to create a biological record of our lives. The brain, then, becomes an historical organ. In its organization and functioning are memorialized our accumulated, synthesized and transformed experiences. And there is no greater period of sensitivity to experience than when the brain is developing. Indeed, as described above, the neuroarcheological record of maltreatment has pervasive and chronic impact on the child. An event that lasts a few months in infancy can rob a child's potential for a lifetime. The true costs of childhood maltreatment will never be appreciated, and can never be avoided, until clinicians, researchers and policy makers become aware of the core concepts of neurodevelopment and the neuorarcheology of child maltreatment. Acknowledgements This work was supported, in part, by the Brown Family Foundation, the Hogg Foundation for Mental Health, Children's Justice Act/Court Improvement Act, Texas Department of Protective and Regulatory Services, Maconda O'Connor and the Pritzker Cousins Foundation. References Ackerman, P.T., Newton, J.E., McPHerson, W.B., Jones, J.G., & Dykman, R.A. (1998). Prevalence of post traumatic stress disorder and other psychiatric diagnoses in three groups of abused children (sexual, physical, and both). Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 759-774. Alexander, P.C., Anderson, C.L., Brand, B., Schaeffer, C.M., Grelling, B.Z., & Kretz, L. (1998). Adult attachment and longterm effects in survivors of incest. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 45-61. Altman, J., & Das, G.D. (1964). Autoradiographic examination of the effects of enriched environment on the rate of glial multipication in the adult rat brain. Nature, 204, 1161-1165. Bell, D., & Belicki, K. (1998). A community-based study of well-being in adults reporting childhood abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 681-685. Bennett, E.L., Diamond, M.L., Krech, D., & Rosenzweig, M.R. (1964). Chemical and anatomical plasticity of the brain. Science, 146, 610-619. Briggs, L., & Joyce, P.R. (1997). What determines post-traumatic stress disorder symptomatology for survivors of childhood sexual abuse? Child Abuse & Neglect, 21, 575-582. Coleman, P.D., & Riesen, A.H. (1968). Environmental effects on cortical dendritc fields: I. rearing in the dark. Journal of Anatomy (London), 102, 363-374. Cummins, R.A., & Livesey, P. (1979). Enrichment-isolation, cortex length, and the rank order effect. Brain Research, 178, 88-98. Darwin, C. (1868). The variations of animals and plants under domestication. London:J. Murray De Bellis, M.D., Keshavan, M.S., Clark, D.B., Casey, B.J., Giedd, J.N., Boring, A.M., Frustaci, K., & Ryan, N.D. (1999). Developmental traumatology part II: brain development. Biol Psychiat, 45, 1271-1284. Dennis, W. (1973). Children of the Creche. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Diamond, M.C., & Hopson, J. (1998). Magic Trees of the Mind: How to nurture your child's intelligence, creativity, and healthy emotions from birth through adolescence. New York: Dutton. Diamond, M.C., Krech, D., & Rosenzweig, M.R. (1964). The effects of an enriched environment on the histology of the rat cerebral cortex. Comparative Neurology, 123, 111-119. Diamond, M.C., Law, F., Rhodes, H., Lindner, B., Rosenzweig, M.R., Krech, D., & Bennett, E.L. (1966). Increases in cortical depth and glia numbers in rats subjected to enriched environments. Comparative Neurology, 128, 117-126. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Fourth Edition (DSM IV). (1994). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Ebinger, P. (1974). A cytoachitectonic volumetric comparison of brains in wild and domestic sheep. Z.Anat.Entwicklungsgesch, 144, 267-302. Epstein, J.N., Saunders, B.E., Kilpatrick, D.G., & Resnick, H.S. (1998). PTSD as a mediator between childhood rape and alcohol use in adult women. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 223-234. Felliti, V.J., Anda, R.F., Nordenberg, D., Wiallamson, D.F., Spitz, A.M., Edwards, V., Koss, M.P., & Marks, J.S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: the Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14, 245-258. Fergusson, D.M., & Horwood, L.J. (1998). Exposure to interparental violence in childhood and psychological adjustment in young adulthood. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 339-357. Ford, J.D., & Kidd, P. (1998). Early childhood trauma and disorders of extreme stress and predictors of treatment outcome with chronic posttramatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 11, 743-761. Friedrich, W.N. (1998). Behavioral manifestations of child sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 523-531. Gould, E., Reeves, A.J., Graziano, M.S.A., & Gross, C.G. (1999). Neurogenesis in the neocortex of adult primates. Science, 286, 548-552. Greenough, W.T., & Volkmar, F.R. (1973). Pattern of dendritic branching in occipital cortex of rats reared in complex environments. Experimential Neurology, 40, 491-504. Greenough, W.T., Volkmar, F.R., & Juraska, J.M. (1973). Effects of rearing complexity on dendritic branching in frontolateral and temporal cortex of the rat. Experimental Neurology, 41, 371-378. Haddad, P., & Garralda, M. (1992). Hyperkinetic syndrome and disruptive early experiences . British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 700-703. Hadi, F.A., & Llabre, M.M. (1998). The Gulf crisis experience of Kuwaiti children: Psychological and cognitive factors. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 11, 45-56. Heidenreich, F.W. (1834). Kaspar Hausers verwundung, krankeit und liechenoffnung. Journal der Chirurgie und Augen-Heilkunde, 21 (1834), 91-123. Hertzman, C., & Wiens, M. (1996). Child development and long-term outcomes: a population health perspective and summary of successful interventions. Soc.Sci.Med., 43, 1083-1095. Hubel, D.H., & Wiesel, T.N. (1963). Receptive fields of cells in striate cortex of very young, visually inexperienced kittens. Journal of Neurophysiology, 26, 994-1002. Hubel, D.H., & Wiesel, T.N. (1970). The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. Journal of Physiology, 206, 419-436. Huttenlocher, P.R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex: developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163, 195-205. Huttenlocher, P.R. (1994). Synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. In G. Dawson & K.W. Fischer (Eds.), Human Behavior and the Developing Brain. (pp. 35-54). New York: Guilford. Kaufman, J. (1991). Depressive disorders in maltreated children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30 (2), 257-265. Kilpatrick, K.L., & Williams, L.M. (1998). Potential mediators of post-traumatic stress disorder in child witnesses to domestic violence. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 319-330. Kuan, C.-Y., Roth, K.A., Flavell, R.A., & Rakic, P. (2000). Mechanisms of programmed cell death in the developing brain . Trends in Neuroscience, 23, 291-297. Lauder, J.M. (1988). Neurotransmitters as morphogens. Progress in Brain Research, 73, 365-388. Meaney, M.J., Aitken, D.H., van Berkal, C., Bhatnagar, S., & Sapolsky, R.M. (1988). Effect of neonatal handling on age-related impairments associated with the hippocampus. Science, 239 :766-768. Molnar, B.E., Shade, S.B., Kral, A.H., Booth, R.E., & Watters, J.K. (1998). Suicidal behavior and sexual/physical abuse among street youth. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 213-222. Money, J., & Annecillo, C. (1976). IQ changes following change of domicile in the syndrome of reversible hyposomatotropinism (psychosocial dwarfism): pilot investigation . Psychoneuroendocrinology, 1, 427-429. Mulder, R.T., Fergusson, D.M., Beautrais, A.L., & Joyce, P.R. (1998). Relationship between dissociation, childhood sexual abuse, childhood physical abuse, and mental illness in a general population sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 806-811. O'Connor, C., Rutter, M., & English and Romanian Adoptees study team. (2000). Attachment disorder behavior following early severe deprivation: extension and longitudinal follow-up. J.Am.Acad.Child Adolesc.Psychiatry, 39, 703-712. O'Keefe, M. (1995). Predictors of child abuse in maritally violent families. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 10, 3-25. Ornitz, E.M., & Pynoos, R.S. (1989). Startle modulation in children with post-traumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 866-870. Orr, S.P., Lasko, N.B., Metzger, L.J., Berry, N.J., Ahern, C.E., & Pitman, R.K. (1998). Psychophysiologic assessment of women with posttraumatic stress disorder resulting from childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 906-913. Pelcovitz, D., Kaplan, S., Goldenberg, B.A., Mandel, F., Lehane, J., & Guarrera, J. (1994). Post-traumatic stress disorder in physically abused adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33: (305). 312 Perry, B.D. (1988). Placental and blood element neurotransmitter receptor regulation in humans: potential models for studying neurochemical mechanisms underlying behavioral teratology. Progress in Brain Research, 73, 189-206. Perry, B.D. (1994). Neurobiological sequelae of childhood trauma: post-traumatic stress disorders in children. In M. Murberg (Ed.), Catecholamines in Post-traumatic Stress Disorder: Emerging Concepts. (pp. 253-276). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Press. Perry, B.D. (1998). Anxiety Disorders. In C.E. Coffey & R.A. Brumback (Eds.), Textbook of Pediatric Neuropsychiatry. (pp. 579-594). Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Press, Inc. Perry, B.D. (1999). The memories of states: how the brain stores and retrieves traumatic experience. In J.M. Goodwin & R. Attias (Eds.), Splintered Reflections: Images of the Body In Trauma. (pp. 9-38). New York: Basic Books. Perry, B.D. (2000). The neurodevelopmental impact of violence in childhood. In D. Schetky & E. Benedek (Eds.), Textbook of Child and Adolescent Forensic Psychiatry. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Press, Inc. Perry, B.D., & Pollard, R. (1997). Altered brain development following global neglect in early childhood. Proceedings from the Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting (New Orleans), (abstract) Perry, B.D., & Pollard, R. (1998). Homeostasis, stress, trauma, and adaptation: A neurodevelopmental view of childhood trauma. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 7, 33-51. Perry, B.D., Pollard, R.A., Baker, W.L., Sturges, C., Vigilante, D., & Blakley, T.L. (1995). Continuous heartrate monitoring in maltreated children. Annual Meeting of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, New Research, (abstract) Perry, B.D., Pollard, R.A., Blakley, T.L., Baker, W.L., & Vigilante, D. (1995). Childhood trauma, the neurobiology of adaptation and use-dependent development of the brain: How states become traits. Infant Mental Health Journal, 16, 271-291. Pfefferbaum, B. (1997). Posttraumatic stress disorder in children: A review of the past 10 years. J.Am.Acad.Child Adolesc.Psychiatry, 36, 1503-1511. Pfefferbaum, B. (Ed.) (1998). Stress in Children. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders Company. Plotsky, P.M., & Meaney, M.J. (1993). Early, postnatal experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin releasing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content and stress-induced release in adult rats. Molec Brain Res, 18, 195-200. Rakic, P. (1981). Development of visual centers in the primate brain depends upon binocular competition before birth. Science, 214, 928-931. Rakic, P. (1996). Development of cerebral cortex in human and non-human primates. In M. Lewis (Ed.), Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: A Comprehensive Textbook. (pp. 9-30). New York: Williams and Wilkins. Rehkamper, G., Haase, E., & Frahm, H.D. (1988). Allometric comparison of brain weight and brain structure volumes in different breeds of the domestic pigeon, columbia livia f. d. Brain Behav.Evol., 31, 141-149. Rhodes, N., & Hutchinson, S. (1994). Labor experiences of childhood sexual abuse survivors. Birth, 21, 213-220. Rohrs, M. (1955). Vergleichende untersuchungen an wild- und hauskatzen. Zool.Anz., 155, 53-69. Rohrs, M., & Ebinger, P. (1978). Die beurteilung von hirngrobenunterschieden zwischen wild- und haustieren. Z.zool.Syst.Evolut.-forsch, 16, 1-14. Rorty, M., & Yager, J. (1996). Histories of childhood trauma and complex post-traumatic sequelae in women with eating disorders. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 19, Ruchkin, V.V., Eisemann, M., & Hagglof, B. (1998). Juvenile male rape victims: Is the level of post-traumatic stress related to personality and parenting. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 889-899. Rutledge, L.T., Wright, C., & Duncan, J. (1974). Morphological changes in pyramidal cells of mammalian neocortex associated with increased use. Experimental Neurology, 44, 209-228. Rutter, M., Andersen-Wood, L., Beckett, C., Bredenkamp, D., Castle, J., Grootheus, C., Keppner, J., Keaveny, L., Lord, C., O'Connor, T.G., & English and Romanian Adoptees study team. (1999). Quasi-autistic patterns following severe early global privation. J.Child Psychol.Psychiat., 40, 537-49. Rutter, M., & English and Romanian Adoptees study team. (1998). Developmental catch-up, and deficit, following adoption after severe global early privation. J.Child Psychol.Psychiat., 39, 465-476. Schaaf, K.K., & McCanne, T.R. (1998). Relationship of childhood sexual, physical and combined sexual and physical abuse to adult victimization and posttraumatic stress disorder. Child Abuse & Neglect, 22, 1119-1133. Spitz, R.A. (1945). Hospitalism: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early childhood. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 1:53-74. Spitz, R.A. (1946). Hospitalism: A follow-up report on investigation described in Volume I, 1945. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 2:113-117. Strathearn,L.; Gray,P.H.; O'Callaghan,M.J.; Wood,D.W. (submitted) Cognitive neurodevelopment in extremely low birth weight infants: nature vs. nurture revisited Stuber, M.L., Kazak, A.E., Meeske, K., Barakat, L., Guthrie, D., Garnier, H., Pynoos, R., & Meadows, A. (1997). Predictors of posttraumatic stress symptoms in childhood cancer survivors . Pediatrics, 100, 958-964. Terr, L. (1991). Childhood traumas: an outline and overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1-20. Uno, H., Tarara, R., Else, J., & et.al. (1989). Hippocampal damage associated with prolonged and fatal stress in primates . Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 1705-1711. Winje, D., & Ulvik, A. (1998). Long-term outcome of trauma in children: The psychological consequences of a bus accident. J.Child Psychol.Psychiat., 39, 635-642

Labels: , , , , , ,